Abstract
Purpose
The purpose of this research was to analyze the unaccusative verb in Modern Standard Arabic (MSA) within the framework of MP in order to answer the research question: How does the single argument of an unaccusative verb, that carries the theme theta role and is generated in the object position, receive a nominative case without moving to the [Spec, vP], which is the base subject position?
Design/methodology/approach
The analysis in the paper was based on the framework of Minimalist Program (MP) which was proposed by Chomsky (2000). MP concerns economy, simplicity and efficiency in the connection between sound and meaning. Essentially, Chomsky (2000) proposes that faculty of language (FL) contains a computational system that interfaces with external systems, sensorimotor systems and systems of thought. The computational system is based on three operations. The first operation is merge, which combines two syntactic objects to form a new one. It is presented in binary branches that project a hierarchical level. The second operation is agree which establishes syntactic relation for case assignment and agreement. The final operation is move, which composes merge and agree.
Findings
The analysis demonstrated that the sole argument of an accusative verb receives the nominative case in situ. This is due to Locality of Matching in which the agreement holds between the [nom] case on T and NP in internal argument of VP. This is because there is no intervening NP between T and the sole argument in internal argument of VP, which is a base object position.
Originality/value
This research shows that the single argument of the unaccusative verb receives the nominative case in situ. This analysis observes the economic considerations of MP as well as respects the UTAH hypothesis, which rules out the structures where the theme asymmetrically appears as a specifier or a complement.
Keywords
Citation
Alonini, T.M. (2023), "Unaccusative verbs in Modern Standard Arabic", Saudi Journal of Language Studies, Vol. 3 No. 1, pp. 47-58. https://doi.org/10.1108/SJLS-03-2022-0039
Publisher
:Emerald Publishing Limited
Copyright © 2022, Thana Mahmood Alonini
License
Published in Saudi Journal of Language Studies. Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode
Introduction
The unaccusative hypothesis (UH) posits that intransitive verbs are subdivided based on their syntactic structures. Intransitive verbs are categorized into unaccusatives such as “fall”, “arrive”, “sleep” and unergatives such as “dance”, “run”, “sing” (Perlmutter, 1978). An intransitive verb is considered a one-place predicate or a single argument verb (Felser and Wanner, 2001). The single argument carries a theme theta role for unaccusative verbs and an agent theta role for unergative verbs (Adger, 2003). Based on the Uniformity of Theta Assignment Hypothesis (UTAH), the theme argument of the verb is realized in an object position (Adger, 2003). Perlmutter (1978) claims that unaccusative verbs occur in VP and cannot assign case. Hence, its internal argument moves, within the domain of VP, to the external argument (base subject position) to receive the nominative case. Modern Standard Arabic (MSA), which is the main concern of this research paper, has the same classification as English regarding intransitivity. In MSA, unergative verbs have a subject that carries an agent theta role and unaccusative verbs have a subject that carries a theme theta role. Moreover, this sole argument in both types is syntactically equivalent to the direct object of a transitive verb (Al-Qadi, 2015). Al-Khawalda (2011) states that the Arabic intransitive, wasala (arrived-he verb) in (1) below, selects one argument, ʔlwalad-u (the-boy-nom), as a subject, which is generated and receives a nominative case mark in the specifier of VP.
However, according to the minimalist program (MP) proposed by Chomsky (2000), unaccusative verbs are different from verbs that have a subject in that they have a little
Research objective and question
The purpose of this research was to extend the application of MP to MSA and to analyze the unaccusative verb in MSA within the framework of MP in order to answer the research question:
How does the single argument of an unaccusative verb, that carries the theme theta role and is generated in the object position, receive a nominative case without moving to the [Spec,
Theoretical framework
The analysis in the paper was based on the framework of Minimalist Program (MP) which proposed by Chomsky (2000). MP concerns economy, simplicity and efficiency in the connection between sound and meaning. Essentially, Chomsky (2000) proposes that faculty of language (FL) contains a computational system that interfaces with external systems, sensorimotor systems and systems of thought. The computational system is based on three operations. The first operation is merge which combines two syntactic objects to form a new one. It is presented in binary branches that project a hierarchical level. The second operation is agree which establishes syntactic relation for case assignment and agreement. The final operation is move, which composes merge and agree.
Literature review
Argument structure
In syntax, Trask (1993, p. 23) defines an argument as “a noun phrase bearing a specific grammatical or semantic relation to a verb and whose overt or implied presence is required for well-formedness in structures containing that verb.”
Basically, the argument can be defined based on the syntactic and semantic roles. The position of an argument in the sentences, such as an object, object, and oblique, refers to syntactic roles. The semantic relation between the verb and its arguments such as agent, patient and recipient, refers to the semantic roles. It is worth mentioning that there is no direct correlation between semantic roles and syntactic functions. As shown in (2), different semantic roles can be held in the subject position. Thus, there is no parallelism connection between thematic roles and syntactic positions (Al-Qadi, 2015).
Levin (1993) points out that each verb requires a specific number and types of arguments to be well-formed in a grammatical structural which is called a-structure. To illustrate, the transitive verb needs two arguments, one is a subject and the other is an object (e.g. the boy broke the window). However, the intransitive verb needs only a subject (e.g. Ahmed runs). However, in other structures, the subject is not an agent since they do not perform an action (e.g. the window was broken) and (e.g. Ahmed liked art). The semantic role of the window is patient, and the semantic role for the Ahmed is the experiencer. For the unaccusative verb, the structure is more complex, there is a monadic verb that has one argument which carries theme role in the subject position. However, other theta roles, such as agent is normally carried by this position. Thus, the UH illustrates the underlying argument structures of both types of an intransitive verb, unaccusative and unergative.
The Unaccusative Hypothesis
The UH which proposed by Perlmutter (1978) clarified that intransitive verbs are divided into two classes:
As shown above, each type has a different underlying syntactic structure. The VP for the unergative verb has an external argument but no direct internal argument. However, VP for the unaccusative verb has a direct internal argument but no external argument.
Levin et al. (1995) explains that intransitive verbs display different syntactic configurations, in which unaccusative verbs take surface subjects that correspond to a D-structure (deep) object, and unergative verbs take a surface subject that makes parallelism mapping to D-structure subject without object.
Burzio (1986) points out that the ability of the verb to assign an agent theta-role to its subject is lost when it does not have an external argument; as a result, an accusative case mark cannot be assigned. Consequently, only the nominative case is assigned to sole argument of the intransitive verb.
However, based on the analysis of Perlmutter (1978), themes of unaccusative verb occur in object positions, but as such verbs cannot assign a case to its sole argument that caries theme theta role, as mentioned above, the sole argument, therefore, has to move to the external argument (subject position). The subject in (3) moves to an external position of VP to gain a nominative case as it is shown in (4).
In contrast, the sole argument of unergative verbs is generated in the subject position at D-structure and S-structure. Therefore, there is no such movement is required in (5) as illustrated in (6).
Unaccusative predicate in Arabic
In MSA, intransitive verbs follow the same classification as English unergative verbs and unaccusative verbs. Arabic unaccusative verb, which is the main concern of this research, has a sole argument NP, which carries the theme theta role and is equivalent to the direct object of Arabic transitive verb. However, unlike direct object which always carries accusative case, the sole argument NP of unaccusative verb carries a nominative case. Moreover, the nominative case is overly marked with the case marker–u (Al-Qadi, 2015). Unaccusative verb moves its sole argument NP to the external position of VP to gain a nominative case (Al-Qadi, 2015; Alrashed, 2012). However, the MP provides an alternative analysis that meets its economic considerations.
Arabic case marking
Arabic is characterized by the broad use of case-marking, as it allows the word order to be varied. Generally speaking, subjects carry nominative cases, objects carry accusative cases and objects of preposition carry genitive cases (Alrashed, 2012).
Previous studies on Arabic unaccusative verbs
The pattern of Arabic verbs is trilateral which means it consists of three consonants. The verb forms are non-concatenative in nature which means the consonants are non-adjacent. Therefore, the argument structure of the verb is determined by the morphological structure of the verb. Abdel Wahed (2021) investigates nine trilateral verb forms and she finds that the three forms exhibit the characteristics of an unaccusative verb.
Based on VP-split hypothesis, Abdel Wahed (2021) indicates that all these forms require a single argument that is located in complement position in the vP and it carries the patient or theme theta role. She indicates that single argument is moved to the Spec of vP to get the nominative case which is c-commanded by T. Also, she indicates that both Classical Arabic and Modern English show the same structural analysis of the unaccusative verbs.
Also, a few other studies have investigated Arabic unaccusative verbs. Al-Qadi (2015) examines causative alternation in Arabic and proposes a different nature of little
Alrashed (2012) analyzes unaccusative verbs based on Chomsky's (1995) approach, in which the unaccusative verb, that has neither an object nor an external argument, places its sole argument as a subject on [Spec, VP], as illustrated in (9).
Another study that is conducted on passive verb which is akin to unaccusative verb in which both have thematic subject that appears carrying Nom-Case. Ben Ayeche (2018) analyzes the passive verb in Arabic based on MP and he concludes that the verb kasar in (10) is derived, as in (11) below, with unvalued voice features which is valued by the VoiceP that carries a passive morpheme [u-i]. VoiceP is a weak phase; therefore, it does not allow the verb kasar to assign accusative case to the argument l-baab because it does not have an agent. Because there is no NP that intervenes between l-baab and T, T assigns Nom-Case feature by agree operation. At the result, the single argument receives the [Nom] case in situ. This indicates that no movement operation is required to assign a case feature to the thematic subject of the passive verb and the unaccusative verb.
MP analysis
Unaccusative verb analysis in previous studies violates the economic considerations of MP. First, the analyses that are provided by Alrashed (2012) and Al-Qadi (2015) violate the UTAH hypothesis, which rules out the structures where theme asymmetrically appears as a specifier or a complement. The Uniformity of θ-Assignment Hypothesis (UTAH), which is proposed by Baker (1988), imposes that each theta role holds a unique phrase-structural configuration in the Hierarchy of Projections as follows:
This can be evident by analyzing three-place predicates such as give and show (ditransitive verb). In the sentence (John shows the picture to Jack.), the subject is an agent, the direct object is a theme and the indirect object is a goal as shown in (12). Following UTAH hypothesis makes the same VP shell analysis extends from one- place predicate to three-place predicate.
Therefore, the theme argument of the unaccusative predicate is the daughter of VP by derivation.
Unaccusative verbs, according to Adger (2003), vary from verbs with a subject in that they lack [Spec,
In MP, as Adger (2003) illustrates that T has a nominative case, while little
In this structure the verb arrived is an unaccusative verb which has a sole argument as theme. According to UTAH, this argument should be merged with the verb under the VP. Then, the verb raises to
The intervening is defined as follows:
In (14), there is no NP in [Spec,
Checking the case feature in situ can be evident by the analysis of passive voice which has one argument that carries theme or patient theta role. Based on UTAH, it is merged as a daughter of VP. Adger (2003) points out that the passive verb is similar to the unaccusative verb in that the passive cannot assign an accusative case to its object as well as it has a thematic subject. In sentence (15) which is derived in (16), finite T bears an uninterpretable case feature [u case: nom] and the argument of the passive verb should be able to agree with T in case features as there is no NP that intervenes between the T and John. So, the argument receives the case feature in situ. Then, it raises to the specifier of TP to satisfy EPP feature.
Data analysis and discussion
In contrast to the previous studies which propose that the theme argument receives the nominative case via move operation, MP provides a more economical solution by providing a nominative case for the theme argument via the agree operation. To illustrate, sentence (17) is derived as in (18).
Firstly, the verb wasala is an unaccusative verb, which means it does not assign accusative case feature [acc] in the sentence and it does not have an agent in its specifier. The NP (ʔlwalad) is its sole argument and carries a theme theta role. Based on UTAH, it is merged with the verb under VP in the object position. Since T bears a case feature [nom], the single argument of unaccusative should be able to agree with T in case features based on Locality of Matching, as there is no NP that intervenes between the T and NP ʔlwalad. Hence, the single argument receives the nominative case in situ. Consequently, the subject of an unaccusative verb is generated in the object position. In MSA, the past tense in T is strong and triggers the verb to move to the T head of TP (Aoun et al., 2009). In the SVO order, the EPP in the T is strong (Fakih, 2016). Therefore, the sole argument is triggered to move to the specifier of T to satisfy the EPP feature as illustrated in (19).
This analysis is in line with the assumption by Adger (2003) in which an unaccusative verb has
Implications and suggested further studies
The researcher would recommend conducting a further research that investigates in how the three different kinds of case are assigned based on MP analysis to reveal a deep understanding of the arguments structure in MSA.
Conclusion
This paper addressed the research question How does the sole argument of an unaccusative verb, that carries the theme theta role and is generated in the object position, receive a nominative case without moving to the [Spec,
To answer it, unaccusative verb in Arabic was analyzed based on MP. The analysis demonstrated that the sole argument of an accusative verb receives the nominative case in situ. This is due to Locality of Matching in which the agreement holds between the [nom] case on T and NP in internal argument of VP. This is because there is no intervening NP between T and the sole argument in internal argument of VP, which is a base object position.
References
Abdel Wahed, M. (2021), “The VP-split analysis and unaccusativity: a comparative study of classical Arabic and modern English”, Egyptian Journals, Vol. 1 No. 3, pp. 38-69, doi: 10.21608/buhuth.2021.59157.1042.
Adger, D. (2003), Core Syntax: A Minimalist Approach, Oxford University Press, Oxford, available at: https://philpapers.org/rec/ADGCSA
Al-Khawalda, M. (2011), “Arabic versus English ergative verbs”, Damascus University Journal, Vol. 27 Nos 3-4, pp. 163-185.
Al-Qadi, M. (2015), “Intransitivity and the causative alternation Phenomenon in Arabic”, Doctoral Dissertation, Arizona State University, ASU Digital Repository, available at: https://repository.asu.edu/attachments/150666/content/Alqadi_asu_0010N_15002.pdf
Alrashed, M. (2012), “Argument structure in Arabic: lexicon or syntax?”, Doctoral Dissertation, Arizona State University, ASU Digital Repository, available at: https://repository.asu.edu/attachments/97687/content//tmp/package-gKyCsD/Alrashed_asu_0010E_12314.pdf
Aoun, J.E., Benmamoun, E. and Choueiri, L. (2009), The Syntax of Arabic, Cambridge University Press, New York.
Baker, M. (1988), Incorporation: A Theory of Grammatical Function Changing, Chicago University Press, Chicago.
Ben Ayeche, R. (2018), “Phase-based analysis of verbal passive in Standard Arabic”, The International Journal of Arabic Linguistics, Vol. 4 No. 2, pp. 271-292.
Burzio, L. (1986), Italian Syntax: A Government-Binding Approach, Springer Science & Business Media, Kluwer, MA, Vol. 1.
Chomsky, N. (1995), The Minimalist Program, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Chomsky, N. (2000), “Minimalist inquiries: the framework”, in Martin, R., Michaels, D. and Uriagereka, J. (Eds), Step by Step: Essays on Minimalist Syntax in Honor of Howard Lasnik, MIT Press, pp. 89-155.
Fakih, A.H.A. (2016), “Agreement in Standard Arabic VSO and SVO word orders: a feature-based inheritance approach”, Theory and Practice in Language Studies, Vol. 6 No. 1, pp. 21-33.
Felser, C. and Wanner, A. (2001), “The syntax of cognate and other unselected objects”, in Dehé, N. and Wanner, A. (Eds), Structural Aspects of Semantically Complex Verbs, pp. 105-130, Long, available at: https://www.scribd.com/document/374426026/Felser-C-the-Syntax-of-Cognate-and-Other-Unselected-Objects
Levin, B. (1993), English Verb Classes and Alternations, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Levin, B., Hovav, M.R. and Keyser, S.J. (1995), Unaccusativity: at the Syntax-Lexical Semantics Interface, MIT press, Cambridge, MA, Vol. 26.
Perlmutter, D.M. (1978), “Impersonal passives and the unaccusative hypothesis”, Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Vol. 4, pp. 157-190, doi: 10.3765/bls.v4i0.2198.
Trask, R.L. (1993), A Dictionary of Grammatical Terms in Linguistics, Routledge, London. doi: 10.1016/0024-3841(94)90013-2.
Further reading
Zibin, A. (2019), “The causative–anticausative alternation in Jordanian Arabic (JA)”, Lingua, Vol. 220, pp. 43-64, doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2019.01.003.
Acknowledgements
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profitsectors.