Abstract
Purpose
The purpose of this study is to empirically examine the relationships between barrier-breakers and customers’ intention to fully adopt digital payment methods (DPMs).
Design/methodology/approach
Survey data were analyzed using statistical methods focusing on hypothesis testing with an ordinal regression model and moderation analysis using the PROCESS macro extension. Participants were divided into two groups of customers in Sweden: adopters-accepters, i.e. young bank customers and adopters-resisters, i.e. members of a formally organized group opposed to a cashless society.
Findings
The findings revealed that only the credibility barrier-breaker could increase the adopters-accepters’ intention to fully adopt DPMs. Credibility also seemed to be an important barrier-breaker for the adopters-resisters, as were perceived usefulness and social influence. Additional analyses showed that the impersonalization barrier reduces the impact of the barrier-breakers on DPM adoption.
Practical implications
Retail banks and merchants can use these results as a guide to what barrier-breakers might affect various customers’ intention to fully adopt DPMs, and to act accordingly. The impersonalization barrier also merits attention when creating an emotional connection to customers who use DPMs.
Originality/value
This study provides empirically based knowledge of the influence of barrier-breakers on the intention of customers, categorized as adopters-accepters and adopters-resisters, to fully adopt DPMs, and highlights the importance of maintaining a human touch in the post-COVID-19 digital era.
Keywords
Citation
Dimitrova, I. (2024), "Barrier-breakers’ influence on full-adoption of digital payment methods", Internet Research, Vol. 34 No. 7, pp. 139-159. https://doi.org/10.1108/INTR-11-2022-0886
Publisher
:Emerald Publishing Limited
Copyright © 2024, Irina Dimitrova
License
Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode
Introduction
Societal developments have made citizens more technology dependent (Priporas et al., 2017), rapidly challenging their behavior (Yang et al., 2015). These developments have influenced the banking and commerce industries as well as customers’ readiness to adopt new digital services (Thomas et al., 2016). In fact, the adoption of digital payment methods (DPMs) has increased worldwide, and cash usage has further decreased due to the COVID-19 pandemic, forcing a move from traditional to digital services (Leong et al., 2022; Santo and Marques, 2022). For example, 95% of companies plan to employ artificial intelligence (AI) by 2025 (Mozafari et al., 2022).
Digital service adoption should be convenient in order to reinforce the impression that new technologies are better than current ones (Laukkanen, 2016; Sivathanu, 2019). However, technological developments often encounter barriers on the way to being adopted. Privacy, security, access, and impersonalization concerns are potential barriers to DPM adoption (Dimitrova et al., 2022; Laukkanen, 2016; Yang et al., 2015). Invasion of privacy, system breakdowns, and lack of personal service are examples of problems that could affect the banking and commerce industries. Such problems can decrease the readiness for DPM adoption and even lead to customers’ distrust of the whole financial system (Shin et al., 2020).
The literature has identified various factors positively influencing customers’ intention to use new digital services. In this study, these determinants are called barrier-breakers – in order to relate them to the barrier concept – illustrating how the aversion to full-adoption can be reduced. Results (e.g. Lee, 2009; Mun et al., 2017; Oertzen and Odekerken-Schröder, 2019) showed that perceived ease-of-use and perceived usefulness are positively related to adoption intention. The literature also considered social influence (e.g. Martins et al., 2014; Tan and Leby Lau, 2016; Venkatesh et al., 2003) as a barrier-breaker. Moreover, the 2008 global financial crisis is still having a negative impact on customers’ trust in banks (Rajaobelina et al., 2019). This may distort the credibility of banking services, and some studies (e.g. Luarn and Lin, 2005; Rajaobelina et al., 2019) suggested that perceived credibility is related to customers’ intention to change their behavior.
In relation to the four barrier-breakers mentioned, the impersonalization barrier could be considered. This concept was first presented by Singh (2004) to describe a lack of personal contact, and later referred to dehumanization, i.e. lack of human characteristics such as empathy (Mozafari et al., 2022). The lack of face-to-face service, i.e. human-to-human interaction, appears to be highly visible in situations such as the COVID-19 pandemic (Santo and Marques, 2022), because bank customers have been encouraged to handle their bank services digitally, i.e. through human-to-machine interaction (Dimitrova and Öhman, 2021).
Customers’ perceptions of digital services are at the center of many of the above-mentioned studies, and background characteristics can affect customers’ intention to use these services (Priporas et al., 2017). For example, young bank customers (YBCs) are much more willing to adopt digital technologies than are older ones (Loh et al., 2022; Rehncrona, 2018). As drivers of digital services, YBCs are considered a powerful group of customers who challenge the market (Nourallah et al., 2021). For example, 95% of YBCs in Sweden used mobile applications (Sveriges Riksbank, 2021) giving them rich previous experience of using DPMs. Therefore, this study uses YBCs as a proxy for adopters-accepters (AAs), i.e. individuals who have started using DPMs, and are willing to continue and even increase their use of DPMs (cf. Planing, 2014). Considering past experience, a formally organized social media group of customers in Sweden opposed to a cashless society, known as Kontantupproret (KU), has expressed negative perceptions of DPMs (Arvidsson, 2019). This study categorizes this group as adopters-resisters (ARs), i.e. individuals who have started using an innovation but hesitate to increase its use, stating that adoption and resistance may coexist (cf. Dimitrova et al., 2022; Ram, 1987).
Numerous banking studies have focused on technology acceptance in various adoption phases. Laukkanen (2016), and Yang et al. (2015), among others, studied technology adoption in the initial phase, while Chawla and Joshi (2019), Oertzen and Odekerken-Schröder (2019), and Poromatikul et al. (2019) focused on the post-adoption phases. Few studies (e.g. Akana and Ke, 2020; Lee et al., 2005) paid some attention to the full-adoption phase. This phase can be described as the end of the post-adoption phase, i.e. when customers have no possibility of replacing DPMs with non-digital payment alternatives such as cash. Such a scenario is of interest considering that Sweden is on its way toward being a cashless society (Dimitrova et al., 2022), and that Thomas et al. (2016) found that Sweden’s infrastructure and technological readiness for the full-adoption of DPMs was ranked third among 90 countries worldwide.
Previous research has often focused on a specific technology or payment method, but not on combinations of various factors, such as ease-of-use, usefulness, social influence, and credibility, regarding all common DPMs. This study also emphasizes the fact that little research has examined the full-adoption phase, highlighting the impact of human-to-human interaction on human-to-machine payment solutions.
To address the abovementioned research gaps, this study empirically examines the relationships between a number of barrier-breakers and two categories of customers’ intention to fully adopt DPMs. An additional analysis is conducted to investigate the moderating effect of the impersonalization barrier.
The results revealed the importance of credibility as a barrier-breaker toward the full-adoption of DPMs. For the adopters-resisters, usefulness and social influence were also significantly related to the intention to fully adopt DPMs. Moreover, the impersonalization barrier weakened the significant relationships under study. These results could be used to improve customers’ experiences in using DPMs in terms of concrete actions to strengthen the credibility barrier-breaker and minimize the impersonalization barrier.
The next section presents the theoretical framework, including the hypothesis development. The third section outlines the research methods used, followed by a presentation of the empirical results. The final section starts with a discussion, and presents implications, limitations, and suggestions for future studies.
Theoretical background and hypotheses
Digital payment methods and full-adoption
Terms such as online payments (Yang et al., 2015), e-payments (Martins et al., 2014), cashless payments (Pizzol et al., 2018), payment instruments (van der Cruijsen et al., 2017), and electronic money (Singh, 2004) have been used to describe DPMs. Despite the richness of different terms, a common denominator is that all these descriptions exclude cash as a payment method (Rehncrona, 2018). Pizzol et al. (2018, p. 634) proposed an umbrella definition of DPMs: “payments made using electronic devices and channels”. Based on that, this study defines DPMs as methods for monetary transactions using digital devices and channels. The study embraces only publicly accepted DPMs regulated by the government, i.e. bank cards (particularly debit and credit cards), Internet banking, and mobile banking. Blockchain-related DPMs are still currencies outside the control of regulatory authorities (Sveriges Riksbank, 2021) and are outside the scope of this study.
Different payment methods are preferred by customers in different countries. In Sweden, for example, less than 10% of all transactions are made with cash (Sveriges Riksbank, 2021), whereas in other developed countries with similar economic conditions and infrastructure, such as Germany, cash dominates among payment methods (Arvidsson, 2019). Among developing countries, China, known for its rapid digitalization (Yang et al., 2015), is ranked just 40th in DPM adoption readiness (Thomas et al., 2016). Regardless of whether a country is developed or developing, adoption of DPMs seems to depend on customers’ perceptions (Oertzen and Odekerken-Schröder, 2019).
The pre-adoption (i.e. initial adoption) phase is when users consider whether to adopt or reject an innovation, while the post-adoption phase is when users consider whether to continue or discontinue using the service (Montazemi and Qahri-Saremi, 2015). When deciding whether to continue using the service, the adoption decision can include adoption acceptance or adoption resistance. Such differences in the customers’ mental state may become particularly crucial when alternatives to the service in question no longer exist. Lee et al. (2005) emphasized the time gap between initial adoption and full-adoption, and Akana and Ke (2020) noted that paying with existing bank cards is not onerous enough for consumers to fully adopt the mobile banking alternative. In the scenario given in this study, the full-adoption of DPMs precludes the use of non-digital payment alternatives.
Technology acceptance model and intention to adopt
The technology acceptance model (TAM) is essentially based on the theory of reasoned action (TRA) and the theory of planned behavior (TPB). The basis of the TAM was the possibility of predicting and explaining how different variables will affect the intention to use a specific technology or system (Davis et al., 1989). Two main constructs, i.e. ease-of-use and usefulness, are found to be important factors in relation to intention. Moreover, the TAM is among the most popular models in the investigation of technology acceptance due to its broad application to various innovations and populations (Davis et al., 1989). Payment innovation adoption research (e.g. Martins et al., 2014; Sivathanu, 2019; Zhang et al., 2018) is no exception in applying different modifications of the TAM. Framing this study, the TAM2 extension proposed by Venkatesh and Davis (2000) – by adding, for example, social influence – has been implemented successfully in both mandatory and voluntary environments (Lai, 2017) and in the post-adoption phase (Yang et al., 2015). Besides various TAM-modifications, Luarn and Lin (2005) extended the model by including credibility.
The empirical results of numerous studies (e.g. Martins et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2015) are evidence of the robustness of the model. Since the focus of this study is a system in which the only available payments are digital, the TAM2 was considered a suitable theoretical basis (Venkatesh et al., 2003).
As an integrated part of the original TAM, studies (e.g. Lee, 2009; Mun et al., 2017; Yang et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2018) have investigated the intention to adopt innovations based on consumers’ perceptions. Chawla and Joshi (2019) found that intention had a significant direct relationship to actual behavior. Considering that DPMs have not been fully adopted yet, the focus is accordingly on bank customers’ intention to fully adopt DPMs.
Barrier-breakers and hypothesis development
As indicated, a set of barrier-breakers (i.e. ease-of-use, usefulness, social influence, and credibility) was applied to two different groups of customers. Based on previous studies, some differences in perceptions between the two groups could be found. The direct relationships between the barrier-breakers under study and the intention to fully adopt DPMs are illustrated in Figure 1.
Ease-of-use barrier-breaker
As a major part of the original TAM, ease-of-use is defined as the degree of effort that a user perceives when using a technology (Davis et al., 1989). Thus, ease-of-use can be a significant determinant of customers’ intention to use new payment methods (Johnson et al., 2018; Laukkanen, 2016; Lim et al., 2022). For example, a smart innovation such as the newest smartphone with its small screen or many features can be perceived as too complex (Mun et al., 2017), decreasing the willingness to use it. The development of most technologies is largely driven by customers’ desire for more convenience (Sivathanu, 2019). DPMs, for example, require no effort: all payments can be executed around the clock, no matter where (Rehncrona, 2018).
It must be noted that ease-of-use is most crucial in an early stage of the adoption process (Šumak et al., 2017). Although young ones seem to have more experience in using digital services than do their peers (Koenig-Lewis et al., 2010), Mun et al. (2017) found that ease-of-use still plays a role in their adoption of new technologies. At the same time, Garrouch (2021) found that ease-of-use is no longer significant in the post-adoption phase for those who have more experience, which was supported by Venkatesh et al. (2003). However, those who show resistance are expected to have less experience in digital technology. This means that ease-of-use seems to be important for their intention to use new payment methods (Johnson et al., 2018). Taken together, the following hypotheses are developed:
The ease-of-use barrier-breaker is unrelated to adopters-accepters’ intention to fully adopt DPMs.
The stronger the ease-of-use barrier-breaker, the higher the intention of adopters-resisters to fully adopt DPMs.
Usefulness barrier-breaker
The perception of usefulness is the level of expectations relative to real performance (Davis et al., 1989) regarding tasks being conducted more efficiently and effectively (Lim et al., 2022). In practice, DPMs’ usefulness is related to convenience, faster transactions, more transparency, and 24/7 access (Zhang et al., 2018). In comparison to ease-of-use, in which the effort of using DPMs is most important, Davis et al. (1989) argued that perceived usefulness is related mostly to the accuracy of a technology. Thus, DPMs will be considered more useful if they are more accurate.
For full-adoption, customers should perceive DPMs as useful enough (Humbani and Wiese, 2019). Moreover, system issues such as disruptions also define the level of perceived usefulness (Lee, 2009). For example, AAs are more likely to adopt DPMs, which offer more advantages than do other alternatives (Mun et al., 2017), while ARs value the functionality of DPMs (Berraies et al., 2017). Although not investigating the full-adoption phase, Lee (2009) and Oertzen and Odekerken-Schröder (2019) highlighted that perceived usefulness is directly and positively related to the intention to adopt an innovation, leading to the next hypotheses:
The stronger the usefulness barrier-breaker, the higher the intention of adopters-accepters (a) and adopters-resisters (b) to fully adopt DPMs.
Social influence barrier-breaker
Davis et al. (1989) described social influence (first known as subjective norms) as customer perceptions in the decision to use a certain technology based on the influence of people important to the customer. Social influence was later described as “the effect of environmental factors such as the opinions of user’s friends, relatives, and superiors” on consumer intention (Martins et al., 2014, p. 4).
However, customers can be affected by different referent groups in their environment regarding the way they pay. In Sweden, for example, use of a popular mobile payment application at university/work can be an obstacle to students/employees who lack DPM experience, which can lead to financial exclusion (Sveriges Riksbank, 2021). Individuals with no or poor past experience of technology are more likely to be influenced by others (Tan and Leby Lau, 2016). Members of groups with common interests may influence one another regarding services such as DPM use, especially if they feel uncertainty (Montazemi and Qahri-Saremi, 2015). Studies conducted empirical tests (e.g. Martins et al., 2014; Tan and Leby Lau, 2016) and found a positive relationship between social influence and customer intention. However, Swedish YBCs have rich past experience of technology and are less likely to be influenced by others (Eriksson et al., 2020). The fact that AAs and ARs seem to have different views leads to the following hypotheses:
The social influence barrier-breaker is unrelated to adopters-accepters’ intention to fully adopt DPMs.
The stronger the social influence barrier-breaker, the higher the intention of adopters-resisters to fully adopt DPMs.
Credibility barrier-breaker
Credibility, related to trust and defined as “the extent to which a person believes that the use of mobile banking will have no security or privacy threat” (Luarn and Lin, 2005, p. 880), applies to all kinds of DPMs. The perceived credibility of financial services is often omitted in most TAM-related research. However, some studies emphasized the importance of credibility for DPMs (e.g. Luarn and Lin, 2005; Mun et al., 2017). Moreover, credibility has the power to increase customers’ confidence in DPMs (Rajaobelina et al., 2019), especially among Internet-friendly customers, for whom the stability of payments is crucial (Yang et al., 2015).
According to Shin et al. (2020), digital financial services such as DPMs are normally perceived as highly credible. Credibility may decrease the perceived risk (Rajaobelina et al., 2019; Santo and Marques, 2022), reduce the perceived barriers (Luarn and Lin, 2005), and increase customers’ intention to fully adopt DPMs. Perceived credibility was also highlighted by Rajaobelina et al. (2019) as a more powerful factor than ease-of-use and usefulness in the digital banking context. The following hypotheses are developed:
The stronger the credibility barrier-breaker, the higher the intention of adopters-accepters (a) and adopters-resisters (b) to fully adopt DPMs.
Research methodology
Questionnaire design
Previous studies in the digital banking context (e.g. Mun et al., 2017; Shin et al., 2020) adapted questionnaire items to be answered based on respondents’ own perceptions, and online surveying was considered appropriate for collecting customers’ opinions.
All constructs were adopted from previous studies. The ease-of-use barrier-breaker items (EBB1–2) and the usefulness barrier-breaker items (UBB1–2) were modified from Yang et al. (2015), while UBB3 was based on Thomas et al. (2016). Following Drolet and Morrison (2001) and the argument that similar questions may have “high costs”, this study focused on the two best-established and most relevant items of the ease-of-use construct to avoid redundancy issues. Three items capturing the social influence barrier-breaker (SIBB1–3) were adopted from Venkatesh et al. (2003), while SIBB4 was based on Rivera (2019). All three credibility barrier-breaker items were modified from Pennington et al. (2003). The dependent variable, i.e. intention to fully adopt DPMs, was modified from Venkatesh and Davis (2000) as a single item. This is in line with Diamantopoulos et al. (2012), who emphasized the benefits of multi-item scales, but suggested advisable application of the single item regarding concrete global questions to avoid redundancy.
Questionnaires can be revised to limit potential bias, for example, by rephrasing questions using less abstract language (Podsakoff et al., 2003). A pilot study was therefore conducted by sending a preliminary version of the questionnaire to 51 respondents, from whom 31 completed questionnaires were obtained. The feedback from the pilot testing was used to make the questions simple and comprehensible for various kinds of respondents. Some of the preliminary items were modified and a few were withdrawn after consideration.
The first section of the questionnaire consisted of a short cover letter and an explanation of the aim of the study, while the second section contained background and demographic questions. This was followed by three core sections containing statements on the barrier-breakers and the intention to fully adopt, using four-point Likert scales anchored at 1 (strongly disagree) and 4 (strongly agree). This is in line with previous studies using four-point Likert scales in the banking context (e.g. Poon, 2008). Likert scales without midpoints were considered appropriate (cf. Nadler et al., 2015), to avoid the potential misuse of “neither” options.
Sample and data collection
The first category of respondents (i.e. AAs) comprised experienced youngsters 18–29 years old, an age range common in young customer research (e.g. Lachance, 2012). Moreover, to open a bank account in Sweden, one must be of legal age, i.e. 18 years or older. Specifically, the sampled YBCs were students at Mid Sweden University. The demographic characteristics and homogeneous behavior of young university students (cf. Tan and Leby Lau, 2016; Yang et al., 2015) characterized this frame of YBCs. Teachers responsible for nine randomly selected educational programs were contacted to deliver the online questionnaire to students in their programs via the university course platform or email. Reaching 913 students, 105 completed questionnaires were gathered after three reminders in the spring of 2020. The response rate was 11.5%. Considering that online surveys typically have low response rates (Baltar and Brunet, 2012), an analysis of non-response bias among the AAs was conducted following Armstrong and Overton (1977) and Levene’s test of equality of variances. There were no significant differences (p > 0.05) between the first wave and the last wave of respondents regarding the key variables.
The questionnaire was also posted on the KU social media page, which, at the time of data collection, was followed by over 13,000 users; judging from their activity, around 1600 of these were considered potential questionnaire respondents. The KU group (i.e. ARs) comprised individuals with varied demographic characteristics but with a common interest in keeping cash as a publicly acceptable payment method (Arvidsson, 2019). Over a period of three weeks in the spring of 2020, 388 completed questionnaires were collected. The response rate for this second category of respondents was 24.2% (i.e. 388/1600), above the usually acceptable range of 11–15% for online platform surveys (Baltar and Brunet, 2012).
Data analysis
Internal consistency was measured using composite reliability based on the Fornell and Larcker (1981) approach. As recommended by Shevlin et al. (1997), factor analysis was used to justify the aggregation of items into factors. Based on the results, purified item scores were summated for each construct to create a new factor variable for each, as is standard in social sciences research (e.g. Shevlin et al., 1997). However, Spearman correlation analysis was conducted beforehand to evaluate the correlation coefficients of the previous items, followed by a discriminant validity test based on the Fornell and Larcker (1981) criteria. Descriptive analysis was conducted, giving an overview of the demographic and background profiles of the respondents. Additionally, variance inflation factor (VIF) testing was conducted to check for potential multi-collinearity between summated items.
Ordinal logistic regression (OLR) was used to test the four hypotheses; this seems to be a practical method for measuring ordinal dependent variables in the social sciences, and when testing hypotheses in a financial services context (Shin et al., 2020). Dittrich et al. (2007) suggested the possibility of using summed Likert scale data as parametric data. The independent variables based on summed item scores were therefore analyzed as covariates in OLR for each sample, i.e. AAs and ARs. Due to empty cells with zero frequencies, the results of goodness-of-fit testing can be uncertain; however, according to Smith and McKenna (2012), this issue does not affect the other types of OLR tests, so they can be analyzed and taken into consideration.
The specification of OLR for both samples is as follows:
Y = dependent variable
β0 = constant
βj = parameter to be estimated
Xi, j = the independent variables
µj = random error
Impersonalization barrier
As indicated, the lack of face-to-face services could be considered a barrier to the full-adoption of DPMs. Impersonalization refers to digital services such as chatbots, robo-advisors, virtual assistants, and payment self-services (Dimitrova and Öhman, 2021). The rapid digitalization forced by the COVID-19 pandemic has directed attention to how the lack of human connections affects customers. For example, Mozafari et al. (2022) noted that chatbots are perceived negatively when their bot identity is disclosed to customers, distinguishing them from chatbots perceived as humans. Due to the existence of the impersonalization barrier may reduce the positive effects of barrier-breakers, a moderating analysis was conducted using the SPSS software PROCESS macro, as suggested by Hayes and Preacher (2014). The analysis was conducted by employing an ordinary least squares regression, model 1, with a 5,000 bootstrapping sample procedure.
The moderating role of the impersonalization barrier was tested for the whole sample for the significant barrier-breakers (i.e. usefulness, social influence, and credibility), on one hand, and the intention to fully adopt DPMs, on the other.
Control variables
As one of the basic demographic factors, gender was found to be significant (Rajaobelina et al., 2019), with the genders differing in their use of DPMs. Age was of interest given the two researched groups of customers. Income (Johnson et al., 2018; Martins et al., 2014) and location (Yang et al., 2015) were also found to be important factors in the digital banking context. The relationships among the main concepts were also controlled by past experience, since both AAs and ARs have already used DPMs. Moreover, past experience has the power to increase or decrease the intention to adopt DPMs (Laukkanen and Kiviniemi, 2010).
Empirical results
Considering the importance of result robustness, the constructs were tested for reliability, validity and common method bias. Table 1 presents the constructs, followed by items and descriptions, and the factor loading per item based on factor analysis and sources. Table 2 shows the discriminant validity, composite reliability and VIF tests results.
Almost all of the constructs’ items had factor loadings above 0.7, except for one social influence barrier-breaker item (SIBB4) for AAs and one credibility barrier-breaker item (CBB3) for ARs. Since lower values can be assumed for four-point Likert scales (Nadler et al., 2015), this study followed Laukkanen and Kiviniemi (2010), and Eriksson et al. (2020), among others, in accepting factor loadings above 0.5. Notably, all items of the moderating variable (i.e. the impersonalization barrier) were above this threshold.
Based on the Fornell and Larcker (1981) criteria, square roots of average variance extracted (AVE) for every construct, compared with correlation estimates, indicated discriminant validity and that composite reliability was higher than 0.7, confirming the internal consistency of items. The additional Spearman-Brown test, proposed by Eisinga et al. (2013) regarding the two-item scale, indicated the high reliability of both samples (0.84 for the AA group and 0.82 for the AR group). VIF coefficients indicated no risk of multi-collinearity, with all items far below the acceptable 10 according to the rule of thumb (Hair et al., 2014). Harman’s first factor test was also applied, explaining 35.1% of the total variances, which was far below the cut-off of 50% (cf. Podsakoff et al., 2003). In addition to preliminary measures such as assuring anonymity of respondents and physical separation of items in the questionnaire, the Harman’s first factor test confirmed the lack of common method bias in this study.
The demographic profile of the respondents is presented in Table 3. All respondents had a bank account in at least one Swedish bank. Females and males were equally represented in both groups of customers. In accordance with the research frame, 100% of the AA respondents were 18–29 years old, while different age groups were included in the AR group. Note that most AAs were in the lowest monthly income range, and that the respondents in both groups had various locations. Past experience of DPMs differed significantly, with AAs often or very often using bank cards, Internet banking, and mobile banking applications, and over half of ARs never or very rarely using mobile applications. Regarding the frequency of cash usage, 42.9% of AAs and just 2.6% of ARs reported never paying with cash. The descriptive results of the frequency analysis of all four barrier-breakers together with the intention to fully adopt DPMs are presented at the bottom of Table 3. All mean values were higher for the AA group than for the AR group.
Tables 4 and 5 present Spearman correlation coefficient matrices for each group. Results according to AA perceptions highlighted the significant positive correlation between the dependent variable and the following independent variables: ease-of-use (EBB1–2), usefulness (UBB1–2), and credibility (CBB1–3). The results for the ARs were almost the same as for the AAs, except that social influence (SIBB2–4) was also significantly and positively correlated to the intention to fully adopt DPMs.
The summarized results based on OLR analysis are presented in two sections in Table 6. Section A applies to AA perceptions, and the only significant independent variable (p < 0.001) was the credibility barrier-breaker. The probability of using DPMs increased if the credibility barrier-breaker increased. Accordingly, H4a was supported. The rest of the independent variables were not significantly related to the intention to fully adopt DPMs, meaning that H2a was rejected, while H1a and H3a were supported.
OLR results for ARs are shown in section B. The hypotheses regarding usefulness (H2b), social influence (H3b), and credibility (H4b) were supported. However, H1b was rejected because of the lack of relationship between the ease-of-use barrier-breaker and the dependent variable.
The values of pseudo-R2 for AAs (ARs) were 0.500 (0.225) Cox and Snell, and 0.545 (0.375) Nagelkerke indicating that 50.0% (22.5%) and 54.5% (37.5%) of the variation in the dependent variable (i.e. intention to fully adopt DPMs) was explained by the independent variables. According to the test of parallel lines (p = 0.337 for the AA group and p = 1.000 for the AR group), proportional odds assumption was not violated.
The moderating effect of the impersonalization barrier was tested for the whole sample but only between the significant barrier-breakers (i.e. usefulness, social influence, and credibility), on one hand, and the intention to fully adopt DPMs, on the other. The ease-of-use barrier-breaker was not included in this additional test due to the insignificant relationship with the intention to fully adopt DPMs. This additional analysis is presented in Table 7, showing significantly negative moderating effects. The results indicated that the impact of usefulness, social influence, and credibility, respectively, on the intention to fully adopt DPMs was weaker for those bank customers who perceived a higher impersonalization barrier.
Concluding remarks
Discussion and conclusion
The influence of a set of barrier-breakers on the intention to fully adopt DPMs was investigated in two groups of customers called adopters-accepters and adopters-resisters. Hypotheses were developed based on the literature, and the results give empirical evidence on a topic of importance for society in relation to the COVID-19 pandemic. In particular, the results indicated which factors can help break existing barriers to the full-adoption of DPMs and that the impersonalization barrier is crucial for eventually achieving a cashless society.
Contrary to previous research (Poromatikul et al., 2019; Tan and Leby Lau, 2016; Yang et al., 2015), the present results indicated no significant relationship between ease-of-use and intention to fully adopt DPMs among adopters-accepters and adopters-resisters. This is interesting, because ease-of-use is fundamental to the TAM. However, the result is consistent with the findings of Garrouch (2021) and Šumak et al. (2017), that adopters-accepters appear to have high expectations and require advanced features. Adopters-resisters also seem to have ample experience of DPMs. In other words, both groups of customers are used to paying digitally. A possible reason for this result is that Sweden is among the top-ranked countries regarding digital infrastructure (Thomas et al., 2016). An additional possible explanation is that ease-of-use varies among different adoption phases, and that it is more important for a technology to be easy to use in the initial adoption phase than in the post-adoption phases.
The usefulness of DPMs was perceived as an important positive factor by adopters-resisters, confirming Berraies et al.’s (2017) and Oertzen and Odekerken-Schröder’s (2019) findings. This could be because cognitive resisters expect DPM performance to be free of technical errors (Chaouali and Souiden, 2019). Adopters-accepters, however, did not perceive the usefulness barrier-breaker as significant, which differs from the results of previous studies (e.g. Mun et al., 2017). This may be because usefulness is perceived as a basic mandatory feature of DPMs, and this particularly concerns those who are less knowledgeable and have less past experience than do adopters-accepters.
Regarding the social influence barrier-breaker, the present results confirmed Tan and Leby Lau’s (2016) finding that those with less experience are more likely to be influenced by people in their environment, which seems to be the case with adopters-resisters. The results for this group confirmed the suggestion that social influence is positively related to the intention to fully adopt DPMs, consistent with Martins et al. (2014). For members of a group with common interests and shared opinions, positive social influence could lead to increased intention to fully adopt DPMs, even though this group seems to resist doing so. In contrast, and as hypothesized, adopters-accepters were more used to DPM technologies, and appeared to be more independent in the adoption process.
The present results indicated that the only significant barrier-breaker for adopters-accepters is DPM credibility. Koenig-Lewis et al. (2010) assumed that credibility does not significantly affect the use of DPMs because of the inability to evaluate security and privacy issues. However, more than ten years later, DPM developments seem to be making at least some customers aware of such barriers. Thus, the credibility barrier-breaker positively affected the intention of both adopters-accepters and adopters-resisters to fully adopt DPMs, corroborating the findings of Luarn and Lin (2005) and Mun et al. (2017).
The additional analysis indicated the importance of the impersonalization barrier in times when the COVID-19 pandemic has forced customers to use new digital service marketplaces. The lack of human-to-human interactions and emotions such as empathy reduced the positive effects of usefulness, social influence, and credibility on the intention to use DPMs. This confirmed the importance of offering personal services in the digital banking context (Dimitrova and Öhman, 2021; van Pinxteren et al., 2020), indicating that human-to-human assistance may help bank customers in the human-to-machine payment context.
Implications, limitations, and suggestions for further research
Previous research has suggested that barriers such as privacy concerns, access limitations, and lack of face-to-face services seem to have a negative impact on customers’ intention to adopt DPMs (Yang et al., 2015). The present results indicated that certain barrier-breakers deserve further attention, not only from scholars but also from retail banks, financial companies, merchants, central banks, related IT professionals, and customers.
The theoretical contribution concerns the testing of a set of barrier-breakers as a TAM-extension. Luarn and Lin (2005) and Mun et al. (2017) suggested that the TAM seems applicable in the digital banking context but previous research has seldom used credibility as a TAM-determinant. This study suggests that the credibility barrier-breaker is important in this particular context, calling for more attention in further research. The study further suggests that the impersonalization barrier can be highlighted as a moderator indicating the need for the TAM to be modified to the digital world.
An interesting result was that both groups of adopters perceive existing DPMs as sufficiently easy to use. It seems that banks, financial companies, and IT professionals have successfully developed devices and systems that help customers overcome the ease-of-use problem (Laukkanen, 2016). This allows them to focus on addressing the other barrier-breakers to further improve DPMs. The importance of DPM credibility suggests that various customers still need to be convinced that DPMs are free of unnecessary technical errors and privacy and security issues (Shin et al., 2020). Retail banks and merchants, therefore, have to maintain DPM credibility and seek to increase customers’ confidence when paying digitally.
Adopters-resisters seem to perceive DPM usefulness and social influence as possible barrier-breakers, indicating that they may need more time to get used to making transactions digitally. This can be useful for retail banks and merchants in increasing DPM usefulness, allowing better everyday payment performance. Concerning the social influence barrier-breaker, marketers and communication managers can further develop social activities and networking events where customers can share their knowledge and experience.
Of interest for the success of barrier-breaker implementation is how the impersonalization barrier is handled. This barrier needs attention when creating an emotional connection to customers who use DPMs (Dimitrova and Öhman, 2021). In particular, vulnerable customers may suffer from financial exclusion. Therefore, a desirable balance between digital and human services requires new service marketplace solutions such as omni-channel services and the creation of bots that are as human-like as possible to make the digital payment process smoother.
Like all research, this study has limitations. As the use of a convenience sample decreases the possibility of generalization (Chaouali and Souiden, 2019), further investigations of panel data covering thousands of participants is recommended. The present study is geographically restricted, so cross-cultural research could give better insights into the topic of interest. In relation to the adoption process, innovation diffusion theory (IDT) could also be considered. A number of studies (e.g. Laukkanen and Kiviniemi, 2010) argued that perceived ease-of-use in the TAM corresponds to complexity in IDT and perceived usefulness in the TAM to relative advantage in IDT. To make stronger theoretical contributions, future work could compare and integrate the TAM and IDT. Further studies investigating a number of barrier-breakers are also encouraged. Single- and two-item scales are recommended when a risk of redundancy exists; otherwise, multi-item scales are preferred to increase content validity. Comparison between various categories of customers based on a more advanced sample than that used here is recommended, as are studies investigating various DPMs in various adoption phases.
Although research (and business) treats customer perceptions as important, such research is limited due to the dynamic nature of perceptions (Hauff, 2019). Experimental methods would address this issue in future studies of relevant subject matter related to DPMs in the post-corona era from different perspectives.
Figures
Constructs, reliability and validity tests
Construct | Item | Item description | Factor loadings n = 105/388 | Source |
---|---|---|---|---|
Ease-of-use barrier-breaker | ||||
EBB1 | Digital transactions are easy to conduct | 0.932/0.924 | Modified from Yang et al. (2015) | |
EBB2 | It is easy to learn to use DPMs | 0.932/0.924 | Modified from Yang et al. (2015) | |
Usefulness barrier-breaker | ||||
UBB1 | DPMs are more convenient to use than cash | 0.844/0.838 | Modified from Yang et al. (2015) | |
UBB2 | Digital transactions can be made quickly | 0.844/0.838 | Modified from Yang et al. (2015) | |
UBB3 | I will not regret if cash disappears as payment method | n/a* | Based on Thomas et al. (2016) | |
Social influence barrier-breaker | ||||
SIBB1 | People in my environment believe I should use only DPMs | n/a* | Venkatesh et al. (2003) | |
SIBB2 | Using DPMs gives me higher status than cash | 0.769/0.709 | Venkatesh et al. (2003) | |
SIBB3 | I pay with DPMs if my friends also do that | 0.728/0.796 | Modified from Venkatesh et al. (2003) | |
SIBB4 | Public opinion affects my choice of payment methods | 0.692/0.726 | Based on Rivera (2019) | |
Credibility barrier-breaker | ||||
CBB1 | I rely on DPMs | 0.831/0.886 | Pennington et al. (2003) | |
CBB2 | Applied security measures are good enough to allow me to make digital transactions in a desired way | 0.868/0.885 | Pennington et al. (2003) | |
CBB3 | DPMs are officially (i.e. publicly) accepted | 0.777/0.620 | Pennington et al. (2003) | |
Impersonalization barrier | ||||
IB1 | Waiting time is long in tele- or chat queues | 0.637/0.720 | Modified from Yang et al. (2015) | |
IB2 | I find personal customer service more pleasant than self-service alternatives | 0.588/0.793 | Modified (reversed) from Laukkanen (2016) | |
IB3 | Chatbots give better service than do bank employees | n/a* | Modified from Yang et al. (2015) | |
IB4 | The lack of personal contact is an obstacle to relying on DPMs | 0.774/0.756 | Modified from Yang et al. (2015) | |
IB5 | I buy more when paying with DPMs | n/a* | Modified from Lachance (2012) | |
IB6 | I want to have the possibility to choose between bank employees and chatbots if in need of support | 0.707/0.642 | Modified from van der Cruijsen et al. (2017) | |
Intention to fully adopt DPMs | INT1 | I plan to use only DPMs in the future | n/a | Modified from Chaouali et al. (2017) |
Note(s): n/a = not applicable; *Items with weak correlations were removed; Factor loadings: Adopters-accepters (AA), n = 105, Adopters-resisters (AR), n = 388; DPMs = Digital payment methods
Source(s): The author’s own creation/work
Discriminant validity and reliability tests
Construct | Correlation AA | AVE | CR | VIF | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
EBB | UBB | SIBB | CBB | ||||
EBB | 0.932 | 0.869 | 0.930 | 1.180 | |||
UBB | 0.692** | 0.844 | 0.713 | 0.832 | 1.216 | ||
SIBB | −0.108 | −0.157 | 0.730 | 0.533 | 0.774 | 2.001 | |
CBB | 0.468** | 0.427** | 0.024 | 0.826 | 0.683 | 0.866 | 1.885 |
Construct | Correlation AR | AVE | CR | VIF | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
EBB | UBB | SIBB | CBB | ||||
EBB | 0.924 | 0.854 | 0.921 | 1.287 | |||
UBB | 0.624** | 0.838 | 0.702 | 0.825 | 1.184 | ||
SIBB | 0.051 | 0.201** | 0.744 | 0.554 | 0.788 | 1.650 | |
CBB | 0.446** | 0.520** | 0.163** | 0.846 | 0.639 | 0.839 | 1.452 |
Note(s): Spearman’s rho correlation analysis; diagonal in italics shows square roots of AVE; the rest of the values show correlation coefficients between constructs; AA = Adopters-accepters, n = 105; AR = Adopters-resisters, n = 388; AVE = Average variance extracted; CR = Composite reliability; VIF = Variance inflation factor; **p < 0.01
Source(s): The author’s own creation/work
Demographic and descriptive statistics
Variable | Value | AA | AR |
---|---|---|---|
Swedish bank account | Yes | 105 (100%) | 388 (100%) |
No | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | |
Gender | Male | 49 (46.7%) | 205 (52.8%) |
Female | 55 (52.4%) | 181 (46.6%) | |
Other | 1 (1%) | 2 (0.5%) | |
Age (years) | 18–29 | 105 (100%) | 23 (5.9%) |
30–53 | n/a | 155 (39.9%) | |
54–65 | n/a | 129 (33.2%) | |
>65 | n/a | 81 (20.9%) | |
Monthly income (SEK) | <20,000 | 89 (84.8%) | 104 (26.8%) |
20,000–39,999 | 13 (12.4%) | 196 (50.5%) | |
40,000–59,999 | 0 (0%) | 36 (9.3%) | |
>59,999 | 0 (0%) | 13 (3.4%) | |
Do not want to share | 3 (2.9%) | 39 (10.1%) | |
Location | Big city | 10 (9.5%) | 113 (29.1%) |
City | 68 (64.8%) | 79 (20.4%) | |
Small city | 15 (14.3%) | 71 (18.3%) | |
Village | 12 (11.4%) | 125 (32.2%) | |
Payment experience: | |||
Bank card | Never | 0 (0%) | 21 (5.4%) |
Rarely | 4 (3.8%) | 138 (35.6%) | |
Often | 33 (31.4%) | 174 (44.8%) | |
Very often | 68 (64.8%) | 55 (14.2%) | |
Cash | Never | 45 (42.9%) | 10 (2.6%) |
Rarely | 56 (53.3%) | 71 (18.3%) | |
Often | 3 (2.9%) | 170 (43.8%) | |
Very often | 1 (1.0%) | 137 (35.3%) | |
Internet banking | Never | 6 (5.7%) | 35 (9.0%) |
Rarely | 24 (22.9%) | 133 (34.3%) | |
Often | 44 (41.9%) | 187 (48.2%) | |
Very often | 31 (29.5%) | 33 (8.5%) | |
Mobile app | Never | 2 (1.9%) | 112 (28.9%) |
Rarely | 13 (12.4%) | 183 (47.2%) | |
Often | 42 (40.0%) | 84 (21.6%) | |
Very often | 48 (45.7%) | 9 (2.3%) |
Min | Max | Mean (SD) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Interval (Likert scale) | AA | AR | ||
EBB1–2 | 2 | 8 | 7.05 (1.212) | 5.10 (1.716) |
UBB1–2 | 2 | 8 | 6.85 (1.433) | 4.26 (1.542) |
SIBB2–4 | 3 | 12 | 5.91 (2.296) | 4.44 (1.899) |
CBB1–3 | 3 | 12 | 9.65 (1.641) | 6.89 (2.008) |
INT1 | 1 | 4 | 2.98 (0.930) | 1.18 (0.552) |
Note(s): AA = Adopters-accepters; AR = Adopters-resisters; n/a = not applicable; SD = Standard deviation
Source(s): The author’s own creation/work
Spearman correlation matrix for AA
Item | EBB1 | EBB2 | UBB1 | UBB2 | SIBB2 | SIBB3 | SIBB4 | CBB1 | CBB2 | CBB3 | INT1 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
EBB1 | 1.000 | ||||||||||
EBB2 | 0.698** | 1.000 | |||||||||
UBB1 | 0.577** | 0.646** | 1.000 | ||||||||
UBB2 | 0.655** | 0.522** | 0.497** | 1.000 | |||||||
SIBB2 | −0.103 | 0.001 | −0.037 | −0.109 | 1.000 | ||||||
SIBB3 | −0.130 | −0.012 | 0.004 | −0.220* | 0.391** | 1.000 | |||||
SIBB4 | −0.230* | −0.196* | −0.243* | −0.217* | 0.336** | 0.274** | 1.000 | ||||
CBB1 | 0.364** | 0.356** | 0.442** | 0.240* | 0.088 | 0.119 | −0.114 | 1.000 | |||
CBB2 | 0.398** | 0.354** | 0.393** | 0.182 | 0.073 | 0.012 | −0.120 | 0.650** | 1.000 | ||
CBB3 | 0.343** | 0.369** | 0.429** | 0.228* | 0.084 | −0.021 | −0.015 | 0.407** | 0.483** | 1.000 | |
INT1 | 0.290** | 0.461** | 0.581** | 0.248* | 0.129 | 0.159 | −0.225* | 0.541** | 0.611** | 0.411** | 1.000 |
Note(s): n = 105; AA = Adopters-accepters; EBB1–2 = Ease-of-use barrier-breaker; UBB1–2 = Usefulness barrier-breaker; SIBB2–4 = Social influence barrier-breaker; CBB1–3 = Credibility barrier-breaker; INT1 = Intention to fully adopt DPMs; **p < 0.01 (two-tailed), *p < 0.05 (two-tailed)
Source(s): The author’s own creation/work
Spearman correlation matrix for AR
Item | EBB1 | EBB2 | UBB1 | UBB2 | SIBB2 | SIBB3 | SIBB4 | CBB1 | CBB2 | CBB3 | INT1 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
EBB1 | 1.000 | ||||||||||
EBB2 | 0.693** | 1.000 | |||||||||
UBB1 | 0.390** | 0.328** | 1.000 | ||||||||
UBB2 | 0.677** | 0.481** | 0.403** | 1.000 | |||||||
SIBB2 | 0.028 | 0.006 | 0.205** | 0.077 | 1.000 | ||||||
SIBB3 | 0.097 | 0.059 | 0.174** | 0.095 | 0.345** | 1.000 | |||||
SIBB4 | 0.036 | 0.030 | 0.175** | 0.091 | 0.258** | 0.337** | 1.000 | ||||
CBB1 | 0.414** | 0.363** | 0.408** | 0.417** | 0.096 | 0.105* | 0.073 | 1.000 | |||
CBB2 | 0.414** | 0.323** | 0.315** | 0.410** | 0.080 | 0.109* | 0.068 | 0.722** | 1.000 | ||
CBB3 | 0.217** | 0.243** | 0.239** | 0.285** | 0.208** | 0.039 | 0.100* | 0.297** | 0.304** | 1.000 | |
INT1 | 0.190** | 0.136** | 0.338** | 0.173** | 0.141** | 0.181** | 0.150** | 0.260** | 0.245** | 0.065 | 1.000 |
Note(s): n = 388; AR = Adopters-resisters; EBB1–2 = Ease-of-use barrier-breaker; UBB1–2 = Usefulness barrier-breaker; SIBB2–4 = Social influence barrier-breaker; CBB1–3 = Credibility barrier-breaker; INT1 = Intention to fully adopt DPMs; **p < 0.01 (two-tailed), *p < 0.05 (two-tailed)
Source(s): The author’s own creation/work
OLR and hypothesis results
Section A | OLR for AA (dependent variable: intention to fully adopt DPMs) | Hypothesis | Result | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Estimate | Std. error | Wald | df | p-value | |||
Ease-of-use barrier-breaker | 0.073 | 0.268 | 0.075 | 1 | 0.784 | H1a | Supported |
Usefulness barrier-breaker | 0.295 | 0.231 | 1.633 | 1 | 0.201 | H2a | Rejected |
Social influence barrier-breaker | 0.047 | 0.101 | 0.213 | 1 | 0.644 | H3a | Supported |
Credibility barrier-breaker | 0.942 | 0.179 | 27.687 | 1 | 0.000 | H4a | Supported |
Note(s): Section A: link function, Logit; Model fitting information, p = 0.000; Goodness of fit, p = 0.831; Pseudo-R2: Cox and Snell 0.500, Nagelkerke 0.545; Test of parallel lines, p = 0.337; AA = Adopters-accepters |
Section B | OLR for AR (dependent variable: intention to fully adopt DPMs) | Hypothesis | Result | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Estimate | Std. error | Wald | df | p-value | |||
Ease-of-use barrier-breaker | −0.020 | 0.157 | 0.016 | 1 | 0.900 | H1b | Rejected |
Usefulness barrier-breaker | 0.655 | 0.160 | 16.704 | 1 | 0.000 | H2b | Supported |
Social influence barrier-breaker | 0.263 | 0.088 | 8.881 | 1 | 0.003 | H3b | Supported |
Credibility barrier-breaker | 0.348 | 0.126 | 7.650 | 1 | 0.006 | H4b | Supported |
Note(s): Section B: link function, Logit; model fitting information, p = 0.000; goodness of fit, p = 0.000; pseudo-R2: Cox and Snell 0.225, Nagelkerke 0.375; Test of parallel lines, p = 1.000; AR = Adopters-resisters
Source(s): The author’s own creation/work
Moderation analysis results
Moderator: impersonalization barrier | |||
---|---|---|---|
Direct relationship | Effect | t-value | p-value |
UBB – Intention | −0.0363 | −7.0560 | 0.0000 |
SIBB – Intention | −0.0161 | −3.5551 | 0.0004 |
CBB – Intention | −0.0330 | −8.3312 | 0.0000 |
Note(s): UBB = Usefulness barrier-breaker; SIBB = Social influence barrier-breaker; CBB = Credibility barrier-breaker; Effect = Interaction terms; n = 493; Model p = 0.0000
Source(s): The author’s own creation/work
References
Akana, T. and Ke, W. (2020), “Contactless payment cards: trends and barriers to consumer adoption in the US”, Discussion Papers (Federal Reserve Bank of Philadelphia), Federal Reserve Bank of Philadelphia, Consumer Finance Institute, Working paper, DP, pp. 20-03, doi: 10.21799/frbp.dp.2020.03.
Armstrong, J.S. and Overton, T.S. (1977), “Estimating nonresponse bias in mail surveys”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 14 No. 3, pp. 396-402, doi: 10.2307/3150783.
Arvidsson, N. (2019), Building a Cashless Society: The Swedish Route to the Future of Cash Payments, Springer Nature, Cham.
Baltar, F. and Brunet, I. (2012), “Social research 2.0: virtual snowball sampling method using Facebook”, Internet Research, Vol. 22 No. 1, pp. 245-267, doi: 10.1108/10662241211199960.
Berraies, S., Ben Yahia, K. and Hannachi, M. (2017), “Identifying the effects of perceived values of mobile banking applications on customers: comparative study between baby boomers, generation X and generation Y”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 35 No. 6, pp. 1018-1038, doi: 10.1108/ijbm-09-2016-0137.
Chaouali, W. and Souiden, N. (2019), “The role of cognitive age in explaining mobile banking resistance among elderly people”, Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, Vol. 50, pp. 342-350, doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2018.07.009.
Chaouali, W., Souiden, N. and Ladhari, R. (2017), “Explaining adoption of mobile banking with the theory of trying, general self-confidence, and cynicism”, Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, Vol. 35, pp. 57-67, doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2016.11.009.
Chawla, D. and Joshi, H. (2019), “Consumer attitude and intention to adopt mobile wallet in India – an empirical study”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 37 No. 7, pp. 1590-1618, doi: 10.1108/ijbm-09-2018-0256.
Davis, F.D., Bagozzi, R.P. and Warshaw, P.R. (1989), “User acceptance of computer technology: a comparison of two theoretical models”, Management Science, Vol. 35 No. 8, pp. 982-1003, doi: 10.1287/mnsc.35.8.982.
Diamantopoulos, A., Sarstedt, M., Fuchs, C., Wilczynski, P. and Kaiser, S. (2012), “Guidelines for choosing between multi-item and single-item scales for construct measurement: a predictive validity perspective”, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 40 No. 3, pp. 434-449, doi: 10.1007/s11747-011-0300-3.
Dimitrova, I. and Öhman, P. (2021), “Digital banking and the impersonalisation barrier”, in Ho C.R., Ng, A. and Nourallah, M., (Eds), Impact of Globalization and Advanced Technologies on Online Business Models, IGI Global, pp. 120-133, 10.4018/978-1-7998-7603-8.ch008.
Dimitrova, I., Öhman, P. and Yazdanfar, D. (2022), “Barriers to bank customers' intention to fully adopt digital payment methods”, International Journal of Quality and Service Sciences, Vol. 14 No. 5, pp. 16-36, doi: 10.1108/ijqss-03-2021-0045.
Dittrich, R., Francis, B., Hatzinger, R. and Katzenbeisser, W. (2007), “A paired comparison approach for the analysis of sets of Likert-scale responses”, Statistical Modelling, Vol. 7 No. 1, pp. 3-28, doi: 10.1177/1471082x0600700102.
Drolet, A.L. and Morrison, D.G. (2001), “Do we really need multiple-item measures in service research?”, Journal of Service Research, Vol. 3 No. 3, pp. 196-204, doi: 10.1177/109467050133001.
Eisinga, R., Grotenhuis, M.T. and Pelzer, B. (2013), “The reliability of a two-item scale: pearson, Cronbach, or Spearman-Brown?”, International Journal of Public Health, Vol. 58 No. 4, pp. 637-642, doi: 10.1007/s00038-012-0416-3.
Eriksson, K., Hermansson, C. and Jonsson, S. (2020), “The performance generating limitations of the relationship-banking model in the digital era – effects of customers' trust, satisfaction, and loyalty on client-level performance”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 38 No. 4, pp. 889-916, doi: 10.1108/ijbm-08-2019-0282.
Fornell, C. and Larcker, D.F. (1981), “Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 18 No. 1, pp. 39-50, doi: 10.2307/3151312.
Garrouch, K. (2021), “Does the reputation of the provider matter? A model explaining the continuance intention of mobile wallet applications”, Journal of Decision Systems, Vol. 30 Nos 2-3, pp. 150-171, doi: 10.1080/12460125.2020.1870261.
Hair, J.F, Jr., Black, W.C., Babin, B.J. and Anderson, R.E. (2014), Multivariate Data Analysis, Pearson Education, Essex.
Hauff, J.C. (2019), “Reasons to switch: empowered vs less powerful bank customers”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 37 No. 6, pp. 1441-1461, doi: 10.1108/ijbm-08-2018-0231.
Hayes, A.F. and Preacher, K.J. (2014), “Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable”, British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, Vol. 67 No. 3, pp. 451-470, doi: 10.1111/bmsp.12028.
Humbani, M. and Wiese, M. (2019), “An integrated framework for the adoption and continuance intention to use mobile payment apps”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 37 No. 2, pp. 646-664, doi: 10.1108/ijbm-03-2018-0072.
Johnson, V.L., Kiser, A., Washington, R. and Torres, R. (2018), “Limitations to the rapid adoption of M-payment services: understanding the impact of privacy risk on M-Payment services”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 79, pp. 111-122, doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.035.
Koenig-Lewis, N., Palmer, A. and Moll, A. (2010), “Predicting young consumers' take up of mobile banking services”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 28 No. 5, pp. 410-432, doi: 10.1108/02652321011064917.
Lachance, M.J. (2012), “Young adults' attitudes towards credit”, International Journal of Consumer Studies, Vol. 36 No. 5, pp. 539-548, doi: 10.1111/j.1470-6431.2012.01119.x.
Lai, P.C. (2017), “The literature review of technology adoption models and theories for the novelty technology”, JISTEM-Journal of Information Systems and Technology Management, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 21-38, doi: 10.4301/s1807-17752017000100002.
Laukkanen, T. (2016), “Consumer adoption versus rejection decisions in seemingly similar service innovations: the case of the Internet and mobile banking”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 69 No. 7, pp. 2432-2439, doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2016.01.013.
Laukkanen, T. and Kiviniemi, V. (2010), “The role of information in mobile banking resistance”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 28 No. 5, pp. 372-388, doi: 10.1108/02652321011064890.
Lee, M.C. (2009), “Factors influencing the adoption of internet banking: an integration of TAM and TPB with perceived risk and perceived benefit”, Electronic Commerce Research and Applications, Vol. 8 No. 3, pp. 130-141, doi: 10.1016/j.elerap.2008.11.006.
Lee, E., Kwon, K. and Schumann, D.W. (2005), “Segmenting the non‐adopter category in the diffusion of internet banking”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 23 No. 5, pp. 414-437, doi: 10.1108/02652320510612483.
Leong, L.-Y., Hew, J.-J., Wong, L.-W. and Lin, B. (2022), “The past and beyond of mobile payment research: a development of the mobile payment framework”, Internet Research, Vol. 32 No. 6, pp. 1757-1782, doi: 10.1108/intr-06-2021-0348.
Lim, X.-J., Ngew, P., Cheah, J.-H., Cham, T.H. and Liu, Y. (2022), “Go digital: can the money-gift function promote the use of e-wallet apps?”, Internet Research, Vol. 32 No. 6, pp. 1806-1831, doi: 10.1108/intr-06-2021-0406.
Loh, X.M., Lee, V.H., Tan, G.W.H., Hew, J.J. and Ooi, K.B. (2022), “Towards a cashless society: the imminent role of wearable technology”, Journal of Computer Information Systems, Vol. 62 No. 1, pp. 39-49, doi: 10.1080/08874417.2019.1688733.
Luarn, P. and Lin, H.H. (2005), “Toward an understanding of the behavioural intention to use mobile banking”, Computer in Human Behaviour, Vol. 21 No. 6, pp. 873-891, doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2004.03.003.
Martins, C., Oliveira, T. and Popovič, A. (2014), “Understanding the Internet banking adoption: a unified theory of acceptance and use of technology and perceived risk application”, International Journal of Information Management, Vol. 34 No. 1, pp. 1-13, doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2013.06.002.
Montazemi, A.R. and Qahri-Saremi, H. (2015), “Factors affecting adoption of online banking: a meta-analytic structural equation modeling study”, Information and Management, Vol. 52 No. 2, pp. 210-226, doi: 10.1016/j.im.2014.11.002.
Mozafari, N., Weiger, W.H. and Hammerschmidt, M. (2022), “Trust me, I'm a bot – repercussions of chatbot disclosure in different service frontline settings”, Journal of Service Management, Vol. 33 No. 2, pp. 221-245, doi: 10.1108/josm-10-2020-0380.
Mun, Y.P., Khalid, H. and Nadarajah, D. (2017), “Millennials' perception on mobile payment services in Malaysia”, Procedia Computer Science, Vol. 124, pp. 397-404, doi: 10.1016/j.procs.2017.12.170.
Nadler, J.T., Weston, R. and Voyles, E.C. (2015), “Stuck in the middle: the use and interpretation of mid-points in items on questionnaires”, Journal of General Psychology, Vol. 142 No. 2, pp. 71-89, doi: 10.1080/00221309.2014.994590.
Nourallah, M., Strandberg, C. and Öhman, P. (2021), “Mobile bank applications: loyalty of young bank customers”, Financial Service Review, Vol. 29 No. 2, pp. 147-167, doi: 10.61190/fsr.v29i2.3451.
Oertzen, A.S. and Odekerken-Schröder, G. (2019), “Achieving continued usage in online banking: a post-adoption study”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 37 No. 6, pp. 1394-1418, doi: 10.1108/ijbm-09-2018-0239.
Pennington, R., Wilcox, H.D. and Grover, V. (2003), “The role of system trust in business-to-consumer transactions”, Journal of Management Information Systems, Vol. 20 No. 3, pp. 197-226, doi: 10.1080/07421222.2003.11045777.
Pizzol, M., Vighi, E. and Sacchi, R. (2018), “Challenges in coupling digital payments data and input–output data to change consumption patterns”, Procedia CIRP, Vol. 69, pp. 633-637, doi: 10.1016/j.procir.2017.11.004.
Planing, P. (2014), Innovation Acceptance: The Case of Advanced Driver-Assistance Systems, Springer Gabler, Wiesbaden.
Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Jeong-Yeon, L. and Podsakoff, N.P. (2003), “Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 88 No. 5, pp. 879-903, doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879.
Poon, W.C. (2008), “Users' adoption of e-banking services: the Malaysian perspective”, Journal of Business and Industrial Marketing, Vol. 23 No. 1, pp. 59-69, doi: 10.1108/08858620810841498.
Poromatikul, C., De Maeyer, P., Leelapanyalert, K. and Zaby, S. (2019), “Drivers of continuance intention with mobile banking apps”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 38 No. 1, pp. 242-262, doi: 10.1108/ijbm-08-2018-0224.
Priporas, C.V., Stylos, N. and Fotiadis, A.K. (2017), “Generation Z consumers' expectations of interactions in smart retailing: a future agenda”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 77, pp. 374-381, doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.058.
Rajaobelina, L., Lacroix, C. and St-Onge, A. (2019), “Does experiential advertising impact credibility?”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 37 No. 1, pp. 171-191, doi: 10.1108/ijbm-01-2017-0021.
Ram, S. (1987), “A model of innovation resistance”, in Wallendorf, M. and Anderson, P. (Eds), Advances in Consumer Research, Association for Consumer Research, Provo, UT, pp. 208-215.
Rehncrona, C. (2018), “Young consumers' valuations of new payment services”, International Journal of Quality and Service Sciences, Vol. 10 No. 4, pp. 384-399, doi: 10.1108/ijqss-11-2017-0111.
Rivera, J.W. (2019), “Potential negative effects of a cashless society: turning citizens into criminals and other economic dangers”, Journal of Money Laundering Control, Vol. 22 No. 2, pp. 350-358, doi: 10.1108/jmlc-04-2018-0035.
Santo, P.E. and Marques, A.M.A. (2022), “Determinants of the online purchase intention: hedonic motivations, prices, information and trust”, Baltic Journal of Management, Vol. 17 No. 1, pp. 56-71, doi: 10.1108/bjm-04-2021-0140.
Shevlin, M., Miles, J.N.V. and Bunting, B.P. (1997), “Summated rating scales: a Monte Carlo investigation of the effects of reliability and collinearity in regression models”, Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 23 No. 4, pp. 665-676, doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(97)00088-3.
Shin, J.W., Cho, J.Y. and Lee, B.G. (2020), “Customer perceptions of Korean digital and traditional banks”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 38 No. 2, pp. 529-547, doi: 10.1108/ijbm-03-2019-0084.
Singh, S. (2004), “Impersonalisation of electronic money: implications for bank marketing”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 22 No. 7, pp. 504-521, doi: 10.1108/02652320410567926.
Sivathanu, B. (2019), “Adoption of digital payment systems in the era of demonetization in India: an empirical study”, Journal of Science and Technology Policy Management, Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 143-171, doi: 10.1108/jstpm-07-2017-0033.
Smith, T.J. and McKenna, C.M. (2012), “An examination of ordinal regression goodness-of-fit indices under varied sample conditions and link functions”, Multiple Linear Regression Viewpoints, Vol. 38 No. 1, pp. 1-7.
Šumak, B., Pušnik, M., Heričko, M. and Šorgo, A. (2017), “Differences between prospective, existing, and former users of interactive whiteboards on external factors affecting their adoption, usage and abandonment”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 72, pp. 733-756, doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.09.006.
Sveriges Riksbank (2021), Payments Report 2021, Sveriges Riksbank, Stockholm.
Tan, E. and Leby Lau, J. (2016), “Behavioural intention to adopt mobile banking among the millennial generation”, Young Consumers, Vol. 17 No. 1, pp. 18-31, doi: 10.1108/yc-07-2015-00537.
Thomas, L.D.W., Vernet, A. and Gann, D.M. (2016), “Adoption readiness in service innovation: the case of digital money”, Industry and Innovation, Vol. 23 No. 4, pp. 353-381, doi: 10.1080/13662716.2016.1156519.
van der Cruijsen, C., Hernandez, L. and Jonker, N. (2017), “In love with the debit card but still married to cash”, Applied Economics, Vol. 49 No. 30, pp. 2989-3004, doi: 10.1080/00036846.2016.1251568.
van Pinxteren, M.M.E., Pluymaekers, M. and Lemmink, J.G.A.M. (2020), “Human-like communication in conversational agents: a literature review and research agenda”, Journal of Service Management, Vol. 31 No. 2, pp. 203-225, doi: 10.1108/josm-06-2019-0175.
Venkatesh, V. and Davis, F.D. (2000), “A theoretical extension of the technology acceptance model: four longitudinal field studies”, Management Science, Vol. 46 No. 2, pp. 186-204, doi: 10.1287/mnsc.46.2.186.11926.
Venkatesh, V., Morris, M.G., Davis, G.B. and Davis, F.D. (2003), “User acceptance of information technology: towards a unified view”, MIS Quarterly, Vol. 27 No. 3, pp. 425-478, doi: 10.2307/30036540.
Yang, Q., Pang, C., Liu, L., Yen, D.C. and Tarn, J.M. (2015), “Exploring consumer perceived risk and trust for online payments: an empirical study in China's younger generation”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 50, pp. 9-24, doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.03.058.
Zhang, T., Lu, C. and Kizildag, M. (2018), “Banking ‘on-the-go’: examining consumers' adoption of mobile banking services”, International Journal of Quality and Service Sciences, Vol. 10 No. 3, pp. 279-295, doi: 10.1108/ijqss-07-2017-0067.
Acknowledgements
This manuscript is developed as a part of a forthcoming doctoral thesis.
Corresponding author
About the author
Irina Dimitrova is Doctoral Student in Business Administration at Mid Sweden University and the Centre for Research on Economic Relations. Her research focuses on financial issues.