Emotion analysis of user reactions to online news

Marina Bagić Babac (Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Computing, University of Zagreb, Zagreb, Croatia)

Information Discovery and Delivery

ISSN: 2398-6247

Article publication date: 14 November 2022

Issue publication date: 7 April 2023

1618

Abstract

Purpose

Social media allow for observing different aspects of human behaviour, in particular, those that can be evaluated from explicit user expressions. Based on a data set of posts with user opinions collected from social media, this paper aims to show an insight into how the readers of different news portals react to online content. The focus is on users’ emotions about the content, so the findings of the analysis provide a further understanding of how marketers should structure and deliver communication content such that it promotes positive engagement behaviour.

Design/methodology/approach

More than 5.5 million user comments to posted messages from 15 worldwide popular news portals were collected and analysed, where each post was evaluated based on a set of variables that represent either structural (e.g. embedded in intra- or inter-message structure) or behavioural (e.g. exhibiting a certain behavioural pattern that appeared in response to a posted message) component of expressions. The conclusions are based on a set of regression models and exploratory factor analysis.

Findings

The findings show and theorise the influence of social media content on emotional user engagement. This provides a more comprehensive understanding of the engagement attributed to social media content and, consequently, could be a better predictor of future behaviour.

Originality/value

This paper provides original data analysis of user comments and emotional reactions that appeared on social media news websites in 2018.

Keywords

Citation

Bagić Babac, M. (2023), "Emotion analysis of user reactions to online news", Information Discovery and Delivery, Vol. 51 No. 2, pp. 179-193. https://doi.org/10.1108/IDD-04-2022-0027

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2022, Marina Bagić Babac.

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


1. Introduction

The interactive properties of social media have transformed users from passive observers to active participants, which provided a significant amount of social and network value to both users and organisations through social media, as users comment, review and share information online (Dolan et al., 2015). To make a better understanding of the utility of this type of information, it is necessary to unveil a hidden relationship between content pages on social media and their users. One of the most significant latent variables that exist in this relationship is emotion. Over the decades, researchers were trying to understand how and why emotions appear and disappear, and how we can influence various communication processes. People always influence others’ decisions in their daily interactions in different ways. One way of influence is via emotion transfer (e.g. word-of-a-mouth) which overwhelms social media (Khobzi et al., 2019). Nowadays many people share almost every moment of their lives online, ranging from their opinions, sentiments and views to multimedia content such as personal photographs or home videos (Kümpel et al., 2015). The massive user-generated content, commonly known as big data, attracts researchers to investigate what causes such behaviour, and how to use the acquired knowledge to the best benefit. And the essence of it is an emotion, which stimulates the behaviour.

Emotions are a part of our internal activity (Plutchik, 1962) and execute a crucial role in decision-making and cognitive relation processes (Shen et al., 2017). Moreover, Kramer et al. (2014) confirmed that emotional contagion is possible via text-only communication and that emotions flow through social media. Therefore, the focus of this study is put on certain aspects of emotion mining as seen from the social media perspective. This study provides an insight into the user reactions to online news on Facebook, a social media service that is increasingly engaged for purposes of news distribution (Al-Rawi, 2017). The focus of this study is placed on assessing users’ activities on the Facebook pages of 15 world-class newspapers, i.e. on user comments to posted messages, their likes and shares as well as their emotional reactions provided by emojis (Angry, Haha, Love, Sad and Wow). More specifically, the relationship between the emotions from the text (e.g. posted messages and comments to these) and the emotional reactions of users is investigated in detail. While there are numerous studies that investigate social media activities and emotions (Tzacheva et al., 2019), especially in the area of predicting emotions and reactions using various machine and deep learning models (Clos et al., 2017; Krebs et al., 2018) and analysing emotional reactions to particular content or topic (Al-Zaman and Ahona, 2022), most of these models leave the semantics of words in word embeddings and emotional values in the reaction distributions, without more in-depth analysis of the relations between different kinds of emotion. There is less research on the connection between different kinds of emotion from text and emotional reactions to it. This study aims at fulfilling this gap by exploring emotional value from text based on eight emotions upon which the emotion lexicon was built (Mohammad and Turney, 2013), namely, Anticipation, Anger, Fear, Disgust, Joy, Trust, Sadness and Surprise (Ekman, 1992) as well as valences Positive and Negative, and its relationship to the emotional reactions of people on social media. The findings are based on multiple regression models and factor analysis that confirmed correlations between certain emotions from text and reactions to it.

2. Related work

2.1 Emotion mining

Emotions are considered a key semantic component of human communication (Banerjee and Dutta, 2015). Scherer (2000) defines emotion as a “relatively brief episode of response to the evaluation of an external or internal event as being of major significance”. There are two widely held families of theories of emotion (Jurafsky and Martin, 2015). In one family, emotions are viewed as fixed atomic units, limited in number, and from which other emotions are generated, often called basic emotions (Tomkins, 1962; Plutchik, 1962). One of the representatives of this family is Ekman (1992), who showed that humans experience six cross-cultural, universal emotions, recognized by universal facial expressions, namely, anger, disgust, fear, joy, sadness and surprise. The second class of emotion theories views emotion as a multi-dimensional space. For instance, Russell’s (1980) circumplex model maps emotions on a three-dimensional space, namely, valence, arousal and dominance, where valence defines the pleasantness of the stimulus, arousal defines the intensity of emotion provoked by the stimulus and dominance determines the degree of control exerted by the stimulus. However, none of the emotion models is better than the others, because all models have advantages and disadvantages. The selection of an emotion model depends on the set of emotions that we want to detect (Canales and Martinez-Barco, 2014).

Over the past decade, there has been a large amount of work with different approaches to detecting emotion from the text (Acheampong et al., 2020; Murthy and Anil Kumar, 2021). Many statistical and machine learning classifier techniques have been developed for affective computing (Banerjee and Dutta, 2015), applicable to both coarse-grained (positive or negative) and fine-grained emotion classification (different levels of positive and negative). Emotion detection techniques can be divided into lexicon-based approaches and machine learning approaches (Canales and Martinez-Barco, 2014). Lexicon-based approaches rely on lexical resources such as lexicons, bags of words or ontologies, while machine learning approaches apply algorithms based on linguistic features (Tripathi et al., 2016). A lexicon-based way to analyse the sentiment (or emotion) of a text is to consider the text as a combination of its words. The sentimental or emotional value of the whole text can be defined as the sum of the sentiments (or emotions) of the individual words (Silge and Robinson, 2018).

As a consequence of advancing the field of emotion mining due to huge amounts of available data, there is a growing number of studies that explore the relationship between emotion and engagement on social media (Luarn et al., 2015; Sandoval-Almazan and Valle-Cruz, 2020). Tian et al. (2017) demonstrated that Facebook reactions and comments are a good data source for investigating indicators of user emotional attitudes. A recent study on engagement in emotional news on Facebook (Choi et al., 2020) used regression models to investigate the relationship between sharing, commenting and reacting to emotional news content that was evaluated using software tools. They found that “people are less likely to share or comment on news stories that convey positive emotions, whereas they tend to react to positive news frequently” and that “sadness is the most noticeable emotion in attracting users’ engagement”. Here, the granularity of Facebook emotional reactions was not analysed, i.e. the reactions were taken as one variable referring to likes, angry, haha, love, sad and wow emojis. Another study (Aldous et al., 2021) introduced time dimension into emotion research by measuring nine emotions (anger, anticipation, anxiety, disgust, joy, fear, sadness, surprise and trust) and two sentiments (positive and negative) to predict emotional audience reactions before and after publishing the posts. Findings show “significant differences for positive emotions but not for negative in the comments among the platforms” implying that news outlets have leverage in steering emotional engagement for posts on social media platforms.

Trying to study more deeply the connection between different emotions, this paper uses a lexicon-based approach (Avdić and Bagić Babac, 2021) for analysing the news posts and comments on Facebook to measure the amount of various positive and negative emotions encoded in the content. Combining the emotions from text with other variables related to user reactions and posts, this study aims to answer the following research questions:

RQ1.

What kind of emotions have appeared in news content on social media?

RQ2.

What kind of user reactions and comments have appeared in response to the news content?

RQ3.

What is the relationship between the observed news content and user reactions/comments?

In answer to these questions, we use a data set of more than 5.5 million posts and user comments from 15 worldwide popular news portals on Facebook and draw conclusions based on the following theoretical framework.

2.2 Theoretical framework

We build our conceptual framework on Katz’s Uses and Gratifications Theory (UGT) (Katz and Lazarsfeld, 1955), as it is an approach to understanding why and how people actively seek out specific media to satisfy specific needs. Moreover, it is an audience-centred approach to understanding mass communication (Severin and Tankard, 1997), which proposes that the public seeks information and specific communications sources to fulfil satisfaction while expanding their knowledge and social engagements through specific media outlets (Katz et al., 1973). To this end, UGT considers individuals as conscious of their consumption, and also that media competes for gratification with other sources (Katz et al., 1974).

UGT has especially been used within research on online contexts, including online games, Facebook and Twitter (Hamari and Sjöblom, 2017). The application of UGT to examine the influence of social media content on engagement behaviours recognises the interactive nature of the media and extends the use of the theory (Dolan et al., 2016). A recent study of the UGT in the context of social media usage (Leung, 2013) has found that users have specific motivations, namely, social motives and affection, the need to vent negative feelings, recognition, entertainment and cognitive needs. The active nature of users in their decision-making and selection of media is consistent with the social media context, where users choose not only to consume but to engage with the media. We seek to understand the impact of this decision-making, and therefore we extend the application of UGT to determine the engagement behaviour (Dolan et al., 2016). Our study contributes to explaining social and affective motivations in the use of social media to express reactions, i.e. what are the gratifications of sharing emotions and opinions with others while using Facebook reactions.

It has been observed that people who use social media receive more social and emotional support from others (Hampton et al., 2011). Therefore, this study aims to investigate how people use social media interactions to satisfy their social-emotional needs, i.e. what emotions people experience and how they carry them out throughout online communications channels. The basic ways that users can react or interact with social media content are by liking, sharing, commenting, and/or by choosing some of the reactions (or, emojis) as a response to the read content.

Emotional aspects of the content may impact whether it is shared. Rimé et al. (1991) created the term “social sharing of emotions” to name the observed phenomenon that most emotional experiences are shared and discussed to make sense of their experiences, reduce dissonance or deepen social connections. Positive or negative emotional experiences can leave lasting social and cognitive traces. After experiencing an emotional event, it is common for people to cognitively replay and reassess the event to make sense of it. Cognitive reassessment of positive emotional experiences reactivates positive emotions, boosting feelings of self-esteem and self-efficacy (Zech et al., 2004). Cognitive reassessment of negative emotions can reduce cognitive dissonance and promote understanding of how the emotion-causing event fits into one’s life narrative (Guerra et al., 2014). Social contact and conversations aid in sense-making through the opportunity to retell the story and make social comparisons (Kampić and Bagić Babac, 2021); they can also lead to the provision of support and solidarity. Thereby, we propose the following hypothesis:

H1.

User reaction to a posted message on social media is stimulated by emotion.

Using a data set of the New York Times articles published over three months, Berger and Milkman (2012) examined how emotion shapes virality (i.e. a number of comments and shares), and their results indicate that positive content is more viral than negative content, but the relationship between emotion and social transmission is more complex than valence alone. Thus, we hypothesize:

H2.

Positive content is liked by users more frequently than negative content.

Furthermore, we draw on the emotion theories in an attempt to find emotional patterns on social media and their possible causes and consequences. For instance, Festinger’s (1962) theory of cognitive dissonance says that the presence of a cognitive inconsistency of sufficient magnitude will evoke a negative emotional state that will motivate cognitive work aimed at reducing the cognitive inconsistency. Cognitions can be beliefs, attitudes, values and feelings about oneself, others or the environment. Thus, the theory of cognitive dissonance is concerned with the cognitive antecedents of emotion, the intensity of emotional response and the cognitive regulation of this emotional response (Harmon-Jones, 2000).

More specifically, emotional dissonance is “a feeling of unease that occurs when someone evaluates an emotional experience as a threat to his or her identity” (Jansz and Timmers, 2002). Emotional dissonance occurs when expressed emotions satisfy the feeling rule, or role expectations pertaining to an emotional expression that comes with the job rule, but clash with inner feelings. Hochschild (1983) defined emotional dissonance as: “maintaining a difference between feeling and feigning”. We seek examples of explicit emotional patterns of cognitive and emotional agreement and/or dissonance, and how they are incorporated through the use of reactions to different types of online news. Based on these possible emotional experiences and their consequences, we set the following hypotheses:

H3.

Positive content influences positive user comments.

H4.

Negative content influences negative user comments.

Here, we make a clear distinction between the user comments and user reactions. While the user comments are explicit text items that express user opinion usually relating to the published post, user reactions are not expressed via text, but rather as a special encoding (so-called emojis) added to each Facebook post. Therefore, when referring to the content of a post or comment, we do not consider user reactions, but the structure of the content itself.

Furthermore, based on Yahoo Kimo News, Lin et al. (2008) created data sets with news articles and the emotions users expressed after reading the articles, from a fixed set of eight emotions. The authors pointed out that the emotions expressed in the news content are not necessarily the same as those expressed by the readers. Rao (2016) and Li et al. (2017) also collected data sets with news articles and user ratings across eight emotions, to predict the predominant emotion of each article and the percentage of votes that users will express for the defined set of emotions in each article. In all these three works, readers express their emotions by choosing emoticons from a fixed set established by the websites. Authors argue that emoticons are related to some specific emotions, even though readers do not express their opinions in a written form (Gambino et al., 2018).

Therefore, we hypothesize over a range of different degrees of positive and negative emotions:

H5.

Joyful content influences joyful comments.

H6.

Surprising content influences surprising user comments.

H7.

Anticipatory content influences anticipatory user comments.

Then, from the relationship between the user reactions and the emotional content of posts, we propose the following hypotheses:

H8.

Joyful content is correlated to user love reactions.

H9.

Surprising content is correlated to laughing user reactions.

H10.

Sad content is correlated to sad user reactions.

Moreover, from the relationships between user comments and reactions, we hypothesize:

H11.

Joyful user comments are correlated to love user reactions.

H12.

Sad user comments are correlated to sad user reactions.

H13.

Fearful user comments are correlated to sad and angry user reactions.

In addition, based on the network of posts (Jackson, 2008), we explore if there is a relationship between the structure of the network and the emotional properties of the posts, and thus, we hypothesize:

H14.

Centrality measures are correlated to user reactions.

H15.

Centrality measures are correlated to the content of the posts.

Here, centrality measures are defined over a network of posts, as explained in more detail in the next section.

3. Research methods

3.1 Data collection and analysis

For the purposes of data analysis, the posts with user opinions from 15 Facebook news portals published between 1 January and 30 June 2018, were retrieved. After filtering the noisy information, i.e. posts without reactions (those with emotion dimensions set to zero), stop-words, non-English words, numbers, URLs and hashtags, we obtained a total of 11,127 posts and 5,505,997 user comments in our data set.

The number of fans on each of the pages in the data set has been recorded on 28 May 2018. The news portals were chosen because of their larger circulation in the USA and their large number of fans (Choi et al., 2020). The number of fans for each portal page with a number of posts and user comments retrieved from that page is given in Table 1.

Each post has its own number of Likes, Shares, Comments, and reactions (Angry, Haha, Love, Sad and Wow), which are emojis that people can click to express their emotions towards the post. Table 2 shows the summary statistics of emotional (Angry, Haha, Love, Sad and Wow) and viral (Likes, Shares and Comments) reactions calculated for the total news portal data set. It can be observed that we have taken into calculation the posts with zero votes per reaction. However, those posts with a total of zero votes were discarded, as they did not contribute to our analysis.

Based on this data set, multiple linear regression models are proposed to test the hypotheses, and exploratory factor analysis has introduced additional value to these findings focusing on individual news portals and thus reinforcing some of the regression outcomes.

3.2 Classification of variables for the regression models and exploratory factor analysis

In this paper, the exploratory factor analysis is based upon a space of posts, where each post is assigned a textual message and a set of variables, namely, behavioural and structural variables. These variables allow for measuring different aspects of user behaviour, and therefore, the goal of the factor analysis is to infer how these variables are interconnected based on the observed posts.

Here, we use the term behavioural variable for the variable which refers to a certain pattern of user behaviour (in our case, observed from social media), e.g. user reaction to a published post; for instance, how many users have chosen the Love reaction to the post, or how many users liked the post. Note that a behavioural variable refers to a collective behaviour pattern.

Furthermore, we use the term structural variable for a variable that is based either on the internal structure of a post, namely, the intra-structural variable; or on the inter-post structural overlap, namely the inter-structural variable. An intra-structural variable refers to the syntax and semantics of a set of words that constitute a post, as well as to the additional properties of these words, e.g. their emotional intensity, valence, etc. In addition, an inter-structural variable refers to the relation between the posts, which is based on their respective structures (that is, on a subset of words that the posts have in common).

More specifically, the value of an intra-structural variable is calculated from particular properties of the words that constitute a post. Therefore, such measurement is highly dependent on the research domain. For instance, to measure the sentiment value of the post, it is necessary to have a domain dictionary that provides a sentiment to each word item, as well as an algorithm to calculate the overall sentiment of the post from the sentiment values of individual words. An example of an inter-structural variable is the degree of centrality of the post from a network of posts, in which a post is a node, and the connection between two posts exists if two posts have at least one word in common. Consequentially, the degree of centrality for each post (or node) is the number of posts to which the post is connected.

While posts can obviously be related based on their structure, e.g. two posts are related if they contain the common word(s), less is known if they can be related by behavioural parameters, e.g. if two posts have the same number of likes, it is not obvious whether it implies any kind of a relation between them, and if it does, what kind of a relationship would it be, etc.

In this study, the values of the following behavioural variables are retrieved directly from social media: v.Likes, v.Shares, v.Comments, r.Angry, r.Haha, r.Love, r.Sad and r.Wow. Here, the v.Likes, v.Shares and v.Comments represent the counts of how many users liked, shared or commented on a particular post, therefore they are prefixed with v. as they refer to viral indices of the content. The r.Angry, r.Haha, r.Love, r.Sad and r.Wow variables represent emotional reactions to a particular post, e.g. how many users expressed a sad (r.Sad variable) reaction to a post, etc. They are prefixed with r. as they refer to reactions.

The groups of used structural variables are:

  • Intra-post: p.Anticipation, p.Anger, p.Fear, p.Disgust, p.Joy, p.Trust, p.Sadness, p.Surprise, p.Positive, p.Negative; here, the prefix p. stands for the post.

  • Inter-post: n.Degree, n.Betweenness, n.Closeness, n.Eigenvalue; here, the prefix n. stands for the network.

  • Intra-comment: c.Anticipation, c.Anger, c.Fear, c.Disgust, c.Joy, c.Trust, c.Sadness, c.Surprise, c.Positive, c.Negative; here, the prefix c. stands for the comment.

Centrality measures are calculated from the network of the posts. The network is created such that each post is a node, and the connection between two posts exists if two posts have at least one word in common. Otherwise, the posts are not connected.

Betweenness centrality (n.Betweenness) measures the extent to which a node lies on paths between other nodes. It is a way of detecting the amount of influence a node has over the flow of information in a graph. Closeness centrality (n.Closeness) measures the mean distance from a node to other nodes. It estimates the speed of information through a given node. Degree centrality (nDegree) counts how many neighbours a node has. Eigenvector centrality (n.Eigenvalue) is a measure of the influence of a node in a network. The gist of eigenvector centrality is to compute the centrality of a node as a function of the centralities of its neighbours (Kramer et al., 2014). For the formulae used to calculate centrality measures, we refer to Jackson (2008).

3.3 Classification of data set items by emotional content attributes

In answer to RQ1 and RQ2, we have explored news content by calculating the emotional values of each post and comment from our data set. The values of intra-post and intra-comment structural variables are calculated using the NRC emotion lexicon and the algorithm (Mohammad and Turney, 2013). The NRC Emotion Lexicon is a list of English words and their associations with eight basic emotions (anger, fear, anticipation, trust, surprise, sadness, joy and disgust) (Ekman, 1992) and two sentiments (negative and positive). These annotations were manually done by crowdsourcing and applied to a set of 14,182 unigrams (Mohammad and Turney, 2010). Based on the NRC lexicon, we have obtained sentiment scores of posts and user comments, which together with other variables of news posts, provide a framework for the regression models and exploratory factor analysis. We further delve into the causes of emotional responses to online news by exploring how and why people behave when being exposed to specific news content.

4. Experimental results

4.1 Influence of emotions on user reactions and comments

In this section, we provide an answer to RQ3. To test H1 and H2, the relationship between the number of comments, shares and likes for each post and its emotional reactions is analysed. Due to the power-law distribution of our data, a Poisson regression model is used to describe a relationship between the variables. The log link function ensures positive fitted values, and the Poisson distribution is typically used for counting data. The set of independent (predictor) variables for the model are Love, Haha, Wow, Sad and Angry, which represent the amount of Love, Haha, Wow, Sad and Angry reactions, respectively. The Poisson regression model for testing H1 and H2 is as follows:

(1) log(Response)=β0+β1*Love+β2*Haha+β3*Wow+β4*Sad+β5*Angry

Here, the Response is the dependent variable and, depending on the hypothesis, it is a number of comments and shares (H1), or likes (H2). The list of test outcomes from the regression model for the coefficient estimates (betas) is provided in Table 3.

From Table 3, it can be concluded that all emotional reactions influence the number of comments and shares, which confirms H1 that either positive or negative emotion is a stimulus to participate in a social media activity. However, this evidence is contrary to H2, because all emotions, and not only positive as we have hypothesized, influence the Like. This indicates that people use the Like button for different purposes, even when expressing anger. Here, we assume that Love and Haha are positive emotions, and Sad and Angry are negative emotions (Plutchik, 1962), but surprise emotion (Wow) may be in some cases positive, and negative in others, so it is highly context-dependent. For the Sad, the estimate in Table 3 is negative, which means that if Sad increases by one unit, the Response reduces exp(−1.730 × 10−5) times. These results are in accordance with Berger and Milkman's (2012) findings in that the relationship between emotion and social transmission is also in part driven by arousal.

The regression model (1) embodies the UGT (Katz et al., 1973) from the conceptual framework because the explanatory reaction variables contribute to understanding why people actively seek out social media to satisfy emotional needs, i.e. people are gratified with the opportunity to express their emotions, and to get an insight into other people’s emotions. The reaction variables propose that the public seeks social communications sources to fulfil an emotional (dis)satisfaction while expanding their social engagements through Facebook. By using Facebook as a means to comment and share the content or choose the emojis, people are also gratified with the reactivation of positive emotions, boosting their feelings of self-esteem (Zech et al., 2004), or handling their cognitive dissonance about the news they have read.

We further propose another regression model for testing H3H7. The set of independent (predictor) variables for the model are Anticipation, Anger, Fear, Disgust, Joy, Trust, Sadness, Surprise, Positive and Negative, which represent the amount of these emotions in the data item content. The Poisson regression model for testing the hypotheses is as follows:

(2) log(Response)=β0+β1*Anticipation+β2*Anger+β3*Fear+β4*Disgust+β5*Joy+β6*Trust+β7*Sadness+β8*Surprise+β9*Positive+β10*Negative

Here, the predictor variables relating to emotional content in comments are c. prefixed, and Response is p. prefixed dependent variable relating to emotional content in posts. Depending on the hypothesis, the Response is positive (H3), negative (H4), joyful (H5), surprise (H6) or anticipatory (H7) emotion. The list of test outcomes for the significant variables is provided in Table 4.

From column (H3) of Table 4, it can be concluded that positive emotions from posts p.Positive influence positive user comments c.Positive (β9 = 0.122, p < 0.001). More specifically, positive comments influence comments regarding the p.Anticipation (β1 = −0.069, p < 0.001), p.Joy (β5 = 0.059, p < 0.01) and p.Trust (β6 = 0.108, p < 0.001). For the p.Anticipation, the coefficient estimate is negative, which means that if p.Anticipation increases by one unit, the c.Positive reduces exp(−0.069) times. If we assume that anticipation, joy and trust are positive emotions, then H3 is confirmed. Both H1 and H3 are positive applications of Katz’s (1973) UGT, and also confirm the Zech et al. (2004) findings of the cognitive reassessment of positive emotional experiences.

On the other side, column (H4) of Table 4 shows that p.Anger (β2 = 0.119, p < 0.001), p.Disgust (β4 = −0.229, p < 0.001), p.Fear (β3 = 0.256, p < 0.001), p.Sadness (β7 = 0.112, p < 0.001) and p.Negative (β10 = 0.193, p < 0.001) are significant predictors for negative comments, which implies that negative content of posts influences negative user comments. However, p.Positive (β9 = −0.172, p < 0.001) is also a significant predictor for the negative comments, so H4 is partially supported. In addition to findings of UGT that people use the medium to benefit from gratifications of sharing emotions with others, here we also find evidence that people express negative emotions to release their cognitive dissonance when confronted with bad news (Festinger, 1962).

The (H5) column of Table 4 shows an unexpected result that p.Anger (β10 = −0.188, p < 0.001), p.Sadness (β7 = 0.119, p < 0.01) and p.Negative (β10 = −0.103, p < 0.001) are significant predictors for the c.Joy, in addition to p.Joy (β5 = 0.504, p < 0.001), which only partially supports the hypothesis. However, this is in accordance with Lin et al.’s (2008) finding that the emotions expressed in the news content are not necessarily the same as those expressed by the users.

The (H6) column of Table 4 shows that p.Disgust (β4 = −0.257, p < 0.001), p.Surprise (β8 = 0.723, p < 0.001) and p.Negative (β10 = 0.109, p < 0.001) are significant predictors for the c.Surprise, which partially supports H6. The (H7) column of Table 4 shows that p.Anticipation (β1 = 0.362, p < 0.001) and p.Anger (β2 = −0.112, p < 0.01) are significant predictors for the c.Anticipation, so H7 is also partially supported.

To test H8H13, we again use the regression model (1). In the second instance of (1), the predictor variables are emotional reactions to posts’ emotional content, and the Response is the dependent variable relating to the emotional content of posts or comments. Depending on the hypothesis, it is p.Joy (H8), p.Surprise (H9), p.Trust (H10), c.Joy (H11), c.Sadness (H12) or c.Fear (H13) emotion. The test results are provided in Table 5.

The (H8) column of Table 5 shows that Love (β1 = 2.533 × 10−5, p < 0.001) and Wow (β3 = −7.652 × 10−5, p < 0.01) are significant predictors for the p.Joy, which partially supports H8. It can be noticed that surprise here appears in the context of positive emotions. According to the (H9) column, H9 hypothesis is confirmed, because HaHa (β2 = 4.034 × 10−5, p < 0.001) is a significant predictor for the p.Surprise. Again, the surprise is in the context of positive emotions, but it can be also noticed that Wow is not a significant predictor for the p.Surprise, indicating that either these variables do not refer to the same emotion, or surprising content does not necessarily imply surprise comments, which is in accordance with Lin et al.’s (2008) findings. The (H10) column shows that Sad (β4 = 5.575 × 10−5, p < 0.001) and Haha (β2 = −1.119, p < 0.001) are significant predictors for the p.Sadness, which partially supports H10.

The (H11) column of Table 5 shows that Love (β1 = 4.073 × 10−5, p < 0.001), Haha (β2 = −4.273 × 10−5, p < 0.001) and Angry (β5 = −1.995 × 10−4, p < 0.001) are significant predictors for the c.Joy, which partially supports H11. According to the (H12) column, H12 hypothesis is also partially supported, because all but Love reactions are significant predictors for the c.Sadness. The (H13) column shows that Sad (β4 = 4.601 × 10−5, p < 0.001), Angry (β5 = 5.317 × 10−5, p < 0.001) and HaHa (β2 = −4.743 × 10−4, p < 0.001) are significant predictors for the c.Fear, which partially supports H13. Compared to H8H10, it can be noted that H11H13 show a mixture of positive and negative predictors, which is also the evidence in support of Lin et al.’s (2008) findings.

Finally, H14 is tested based on a regression model (1), and H15 is based on a regression model (2). In both settings, the dependent variable is a centrality measure, respectively. The independent variables are emotional reactions to the posts in testing H14, and posts’ emotions in testing H15. From regression outcomes shown in Table 6, it can be observed that for betweenness and degree centralities, both hypotheses are confirmed, because all predictor variables are significant for the dependent variable. For closeness centrality (omitted from the table), both H14 and H15 are rejected as none of the predictors are significant. For eigenvalue centrality, H15 is partially supported as only p.Joy (β9 = −0.129, p < 0.001), p.Surprise (β8 = 0.225, p < 0.001), p.Trust (β6 = 0.122, p < 0.001) and p.Positive (β10 = 0.075, p < 0.001) are significant predictors. Overall, it is safe to conclude that both hypotheses are partially supported.

4.2 Influence of emotions on individual news portals

In this section, exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was used to find out the underlying factors in the motivation for participating in online news portals. The answers to the following questions for each of the individual news portals from the data set are given:

  • What is the number of underlying patterns (factors), how many factors best fit the data and what are the underlying pieces?

  • How do the variables group together?

  • Which variables can be eliminated as not being important?

The following EFA analyses were conducted using guidelines outlined in Preacher et al. (2003). Bartlett’s test indicated correlation adequacy, and the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin test indicated sampling adequacy for each EFA instance. Maximum likelihood estimation was used with direct oblimin rotation (a non-orthogonal oblique solution) because of the expected factor correlation.

The results of the 15-factor analyses, each corresponding to a single news portal, are summarized in Figure 1. The colours in the figure are used only to distinguish between different factors in a single column, so the same colour in different columns does not imply that the factors have something in common. However, the variables of the same colour in the same column belong to the same factor.

It can be noticed that there exist no pair of columns identical by factor variables, which indicates different user behaviour on these portals. It can be noticed that for the majority of the portals the variables are grouped into five factors, with the exception of Yahoo, which has six factors, and BBC, CNN, Fox and New York Times, which have four factors. It is obvious from Figure 1 that none of the viral or reaction variables appeared in a common factor with any of the post/comment emotion variables nor any of the network centrality variables.

4.2.1 Factorization of viral and reaction variables

From Figure 1, it can be noticed that the viral variables are grouped along with reaction variables for all factors, i.e. none of the factors is consisted of only viral variables, which is evidence in support of H1, that different emotions stimulate social media activities.

The Angry reaction is the least factorized variable in the figure (e.g. it only appears on Huffington Post, WA Post and Yahoo pages as a constituent variable), suggesting that people either avoid writing about what makes them angry, or the content on social media has no intention to evoke such emotional reaction. Based on emotion dissonance (Jansz and Timmers, 2002), people avoid participating in discussions that make them feel bad about themselves, or what is in contrast to their expectations. Expectation dissonance creates these unpleasant emotions (Hochschild, 1983), and people a priori avoid reading articles that are not in accordance with their beliefs. On the other hand, an angry emotion does not correlate to our set of other variables, so it might be the case that either online news does not cause such emotion, or it is caused by other online content which is not significantly present in online news.

In the rest of the section, we extract factors from Figure 1, where their ordering is completely at random, and the number dedicated to each factor has no other meaning, but the unique name of a factor. In addition, it is important to extract them to find how much of the factors repeat, only in terms of variable groupings, and not by any factor loading values.

The r.Angry variable is factorized as follows:

  • Factor r1: v.Comments, r.Angry, r.Sad, r.Wow (Huffington);

  • Factor r2: v.Comments, v.Shares, r.Angry, r.Sad, r.Wow (Washington); and

  • Factor r3: r.Angry, r.Sad (Yahoo).

From these factors, it is safe to conclude that sad emotion on these pages is closely tied to anger. There is also a certain connection to surprise. On Huffington and Washington Post pages people feel free to comment about the bad news, while on the Yahoo page people express their negative feelings about the content, but they do not share or comment much. If we choose to interpret factors r1, r2 and r3 in terms of what might have caused such user behaviour, we may recall the theory of cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1962), that a negative emotional state initiates a reduction of cognitive inconsistency. So, we perceive the behaviour as a direct consequence of the dissonance between the expected and observed, and an action of commenting, sharing or choosing a particular reaction (in this case, from a class of negatives) is the act of actual reduction of such conflict. In addition, both angry and surprise emotions include much of the arousal substance of the emotion Berger and Milkman (2012), which complements the finding of dissonance reduction by exhibiting negative emotions.

According to the factorization of the r.Haha reaction, it can be observed that humour is only present in four factors (at ABC, CBS, NYT and Wallstreet pages), indicating that such reaction is not a very common reaction on news pages. The explanations may be the same as for the angry emotion since both emotions have strong arousal and valence features (Russell, 1980).

From the factors table, it can be seen that the r.Haha variable is tied to:

  • Factor r4: v.Likes, v.Comments, v.Shares, r. Haha, r.Love, r.Wow (ABC, Wallstreet);

  • Factor r5: v.Comments, v.Shares, r.Haha, r.Sad (CBS); and

  • Factor r6: v.Likes, r.Haha, r.Love (NBC).

From these factors, it can be seen that r.Haha relates to positive emotions at ABC, NBC and Wallstreet, so it can be concluded that either these pages offer more positive content, or present the content in such fashion. People also feel free to express their positive emotions on these pages, both by commenting, sharing and liking the content, as well as choosing these particular emotions from the reaction list.

The factors r4 and r6 contribute to H2 that people feel good about the content that is in accordance with their expectations, i.e. without cognitive dissonance, and indicate that people like to replay and take part in the activities that confirm their self-satisfaction (Zech et al., 2004).

It seems unexpected to observe a factor r5 that includes both r.Haha and r.Sad variables. However, the interpretation may be related to either different perceptions of the content by different users, or it may be the case that the CBS page publishes ironic, sarcastic or similar ambiguous content that amuses people (Calderon and Kuo, 2019). The possible explanation of such a pattern is also in strong polarity of the content, for instance, when people have opposite attitudes towards certain entities, or events (e.g. sporting events such as soccer competitions, or political elections with opposite candidates or parties). This would further imply that such pages attract readers of different profiles and that a page does not prefer any particular side of the target issues.

The r.Wow reaction is factorized (besides for the factors r1, r2, r4) as follows:

  • Factor r7: v.Likes, v.Comments, v.Shares, r.Love, and r.Wow (appeared four times).

It can be noticed from here that the r.Wow has appeared six times in factors with positive emotions, in contrast to two times in factors with negative emotions.

The r.Sad reaction is factorized (besides for the factors r1, r2, r3 and r5) as follows:

  • Factor r8: v.Shares, v.Comments, r.Sad (Guardian); and

  • Factor r9: v.Likes, v.Comments, v.Shares, r.Love, r.Sad (USA).

Although it may seem unusual to observe both r.Love and r.Sad in the same factor, it is actually a common reaction when people read about sad content that they show their compassion with a love reaction. Therefore, the difference between the factors r8 and r9 might be in the intensity of sad emotions. These findings are in accordance with H5.

4.2.2 Factorization of the post and comment variables

While it is expected that the positive content of posts or comments is grouped into the same factor, it is much more interesting to notice if positive posts influence positive comments, or if negative posts influence negative comments (H3 and H4). According to the emotions inferred from the text, the posts are factorized as shown in Table 7:

Here, factor p1 appeared at seven pages, while p2 appeared at nine pages. The factors p1, p2 and p4 are expected to appear because they group variables from a similar context, e.g. positive/negative emotions with different intensities are grouped together. These findings are in line with H3 and H4 in that people react positively to what makes them feel good about themselves and reinforces their positive attitudes towards a particular entity or event, and people react negatively when they experience unpleasant observation that is not in accordance with their beliefs or expectations (Festinger, 1962). Yet, it is interesting to observe the factor p3, present on one page (ABC), that groups a collection of positive emotions with p.Anger, which may indicate the presence of ambiguity or polarity in a post.

The factors that combine posts and comments are shown in Table 8:

Groups of variables in factors pc3pc9 unveil the same pattern of user behaviour about negative content, as stated in H4, that people write negative comments related to negative content. This supports the theories of cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1962) and UGT (Katz et al., 1973) as it reinforces the strength of social media as a means of reducing the dissonance while allowing for writing different opinions. In addition, factors pc1 and pc2 are in accordance with H3, that people write positively when exposed to positive content. This again allows for the interpretation of UGT that people are gratified with opportunities to share their positive experiences and boost their positive emotions.

It can be also noticed from the factor list that surprise is correlated to positive emotions on BBC page.

The comments are factorized as shown in Table 9:

Here, five factors (c3c7) show a mixture of positive and negative emotions in user comments, which indicates that, on these news portals, people perceive the same content differently and that people feel free to express their different opinions. More in-depth analysis of these comments (based on actual topics) would discover if people disagree among themselves or about the content. In factor c1, the surprise is grouped with positive emotions, and in factor c8, it is grouped with negative emotions, which indicates that surprise is a highly context-dependent emotion.

4.2.3 Factorization of the network variables

The network centrality variables are factorized as shown in Table 10:

The grouping of centrality measures is expected due to their methods of calculation and interpretation, i.e. they relate to the structure of the network of posts. The most frequent factor that appeared 8 times is n1, but it did not correlate to any other type of variable except for the network ones.

It is interesting to observe that on the CBS page, the centrality of posts relates to trust, indicating that CBS mostly uses (or, chooses) the set of words labelled with the trust emotion when publishing online (factor n4). Then, ABC and BBC pages publish in such a way that the centrality of posts is related to negative comments, which indicates that the most frequent words used on these pages attract many negative comments, or that the most central posts tend to be the viral ones. On the other hand, Wallstreet and Washington Post show that the centrality of posts relates to positive emotions like trust and surprise, which might imply that the most frequently used words on these pages are positive, and present mostly either the positive aspects of news or use the positive approach in presenting any kind of news (factors n5 and n6).

Although not present for each news page, certain connections between the network structure of the posts and emotion variables did appear, so they give us an impression of what kind of news, or indirectly, what news writing style appeared online at individual news portals.

5. Discussion

5.1 Conclusions

On social media, the goal of publishing a post is to attract an audience by providing value, or gratification, through its content. Therefore, a post should be designed in a way that creates value for individual users to build a stronger level of engagement and facilitate value outcomes (Malthouse et al., 2013). The essence of achieving this goal is in emotion. Depending on the evoked emotions, the reader of the content decides on how to continue with his/her actions, i.e. to engage more deeply with the medium, or to leave. Therefore, we have analysed the user reactions and comments to find out what kind of emotions appear in response to news on social media, and how they influence user behaviour. The theoretical model and corresponding hypotheses addressed the dynamic nature of how social media content impacts user participation.

Our results have confirmed a subset of the hypotheses, such as that emotion is a stimulus to a particular pattern of behaviour (H1), or that positive emotions usually influence positive comments (H3, H8), while negative emotions influence negative comments (H4, H10). When people read news about successful persons or events, sometimes they identify themselves with success and reinforce a positive self-image (Zech et al., 2004). However, the most interesting findings are those of unexpected behaviour. For instance, testing H5 has unveiled that joyful comments were not evoked only by joyful, but also sad and angry posts. One of the possible explanations is that people sometimes feel better about themselves when reading bad news because they feel relief that they did not experience the bad story themselves (Rimé et al., 1991). In opposite, it might be the case that people do not enjoy so much listening about positive events going on, because they feel annoyed and inferior in some of the descriptions where they cannot identify themselves. H2 implies that positive reactions do not necessarily mean that something good has happened. For instance, people are sometimes compassioned when something bad happens, and they express their empathy via positive rather than negative reactions (H11H13). Therefore, our findings reinforce Lin et al.’s (2008) conclusions that the emotions expressed in the news content are not necessarily the same as those expressed by the users. It is also interesting to observe that surprise comments were impacted mostly by negative posts (H6).

Regarding H14 and H15, some of the centrality measures (e.g. betweenness and degree centrality) have shown a significant correlation with emotions (e.g. trust), even at some individual news portals, which might have useful implications for the designers of online content. The network of posts is based on the textual structure of posts, so choosing a particular set of words in presenting a news event may evoke a predicted set of emotions (e.g. trust, surprise, positive or negative). Overall, certain centrality measures are correlated with emotions (H14), which is a strong indication of how vocabulary and structure of sentences might have far-reaching consequences (H15) such as an increase in news consumption or modification of user views or sentiments about particular event or entity. Our findings are promising for future studies, which can lead to the development of new hypotheses and theoretical models about online social interactions in social media.

5.2 Theoretical implications

As social media grow, and users increasingly express their opinions and comments on various topics, automatic emotion detection from the text has attracted growing attention due to its potentially useful applications (Woodruff et al., 2020; Sandoval-Almazan and Valle-Cruz, 2020; Puh and Bagić Babac, 2022).

By providing fine-grained assessments of users’ activities, the theoretical contributions of this study are twofold. On one hand, an insight into the emotional reactions of users based on the emotion theories is shown, which provides the marketers and decision-makers with an emotional framework to shape online content according to users’ needs (Luarn et al., 2015). Our theoretical contributions may be extended to other domains of interest, beyond the scope of the news domain, e.g. politics, sports, technology. On the other hand, from the presented factor analysis, it is possible to detect details that make one news portal different from the other. By exploring the user reactions in the direction of readership profiling, we may expand our understanding of how and under what circumstances the design of online news spaces helps (or does not help) to cultivate participatory practices among users (Almgren and Olsson, 2016). In addition, the data set as well as the findings from this study may serve data scientists in developing predictive models of user behaviour (Preston et al., 2021).

5.3 Practical implications

Our findings have several practical implications for social media strategies that can help marketers to understand user participation (Dwivedi et al., 2019). Based on the revealed relationship between the various emotional contents and user reactions to it, marketers can structure communication content in such a way that it promotes positive engagement behaviours (Gummerus et al., 2012). Moreover, brand managers that use social media platforms can be guided by this research in deciding which characteristics of content to place within posts to elicit favourable behavioural responses among users (Han, 2021; Molina et al., 2020; Moussa, 2019; Shen et al., 2017).

Reliable emotion detection can help develop powerful human-computer interaction devices. Based on the results presented in this study, emotion detectors can be developed, implemented and tested for various purposes and domains of use. Moreover, the use of machine learning (Cvitanović and Bagić Babac, 2022) and deep emotional analysis of data (Kawade and Waghmare, 2017) could reveal further interesting insights into human nature and behaviour.

5.4 Limitations and future research

While this study has provided interesting insights into user behaviour in an online news setting, it has limitations that need to be addressed. Our observations are based on different news portals in terms of journal policy as well as the targeted audience. For instance, according to Glader (2017), the New York Times’s editorial page and some of its news coverage take “a left-leaning, progressive view of the world”. Then, Wallstreet’s “editorial page is a bastion of American free-market conservatism”. Washington Post “is arguably the most forward-thinking right now in trying new digital strategies that have boosted readership”. The BBC is the global standard-bearer for excellence in broadcast radio and TV journalism. In addition, some of the chosen news portals are among the most popular news brands in the USA (Verto Analytics, 2018), e.g. Fox News, NY Times, CNN, WA Post, USA Today, The Huffington Post and CBS News. Our selection of journals is limited in many aspects, so future avenues of this research might consider a wider range of editorial policies and journals, cultural or language areas, or websites beyond social networks.

Future research could theorise and examine the influence of social media content on a wider range of cognitive and affective dimensions. Also, more in-depth text mining would show the relationship between the topics of posts and user reactions, or topics from the comments. This would provide an even more comprehensive understanding of the engagement attributed to social media content, and consequently, could be a better predictor of future behaviour. Moreover, it would be interesting to compare user reactions or pulse among different cultures, regions or countries.

Figures

Factor loadings per factors for the news portals

Figure 1

Factor loadings per factors for the news portals

A number of posts and comments appeared on Facebook news pages between 1 January and 30 June 2018, and a number of fans on 28 May 2018

News portal Fans Posts Comments
ABC News 12,987,064 600 387,393
BBC News 46,663,616 854 688,712
CBS News 4,750,373 654 320,932
CNN 30,251,773 795 1,062,187
Fox News 16,414,115 428 935,135
Huffington Post 9,858,289 678 216,253
Los Angeles Times 2,731,700 857 77,845
NBC News 9,715,723 772 355,936
New York Times 15,707,014 1.023 283,640
NPR 6,300,058 750 247,988
The Guardian 7,756,557 741 146,455
USA Today 7,580,084 761 233,928
Wall Street Journal 6,201,146 708 32,416
Washington Post 6,161,639 838 157,813
Yahoo 14,547,436 668 359,364

Summary statistics of the news data set by emotional and viral reactions

Dimension Summary statistics
Min. 1st Qu. Median Mean 3rd Qu. Max
Angry 0 1 8 266 77 41.580
Haha 0 3 14 183 77 46.303
Love 0 5 27 438 159 109.328
Sad 0 1 8 221 58 50.178
Wow 0 7 26 183 96 24.599
Like 4 182 530 2.285 1.756 25.483
Share 0 53 177 1.428 662 351.082
Comment 1 37 123 530 418 47.789

Regression results detailing the influence of reactions on comments, shares and likes

Independent variables Dependent variable
Comments Shares Likes
Love 6.056e + 00 *** 4.226e-05 *** 5.673e-05 ***
HaHa 3.765e-05 *** 1.123e-04 *** 8.884e-05 ***
Wow 1.178e-04 *** 1.819e-04 *** 1.184e-04 ***
Sad 2.388e-05 *** 7.886e-05 *** -1.730e-05 ***
Angry 1.187e-04 *** 8.849e-05 *** 3.698e-05 ***
Note:

Significance codes: ***p < 0.001;

**p < 0.01;

*p < 0.05

Regression results from testing the relationship between the posts and comments

Independent variables Dependent variable
c.Positive (H3) c.Negative (H4) c.Joy (H5) c.Surprise (H6) c.Anticipation (H7)
p.Anger −0.071 0.119 *** −0.188 *** −0.109 −0.112 **
p.Anticipation −0.069 *** −0.012 −0.004 −0.054 0.362 ***
p.Disgust −0.052 −0.229 *** 0.020 −0.257 *** −0.055
p.Fear −0.053 0.256 *** −0.083 0.048 0.009
p.Joy 0.059 ** −0.108 0.540 *** −0.034 0.037
p.Sadness −0.007 0.112 *** 0.119 ** −0.057 0.055
p.Surprise 0.050 −0.062 −0.051 0.723 *** 0.029
p.Trust 0.108 *** 0.048 0.034 0.015 0.030
p.Negative −0.036 0.193 *** −0.103 *** 0.109 *** −0.026
p.Positive 0.122 *** −0.172 *** −0.027 −0.067 −0.057
Note:

Significance codes: ***p < 0.001;

**p < 0.01;

*p < 0.05

Regression results from testing the relationship between the posts/comments’ emotions and reactions

Independent variables Dependent variable
p.Joy (H8) p.Surprise (H9) p.Sadness (H10) c.Joy (H11) c.Sadness (H12) c.Fear (H13)
Love 2.533e-05*** 2.773e-06 5.235e-06 4.073e-05*** −4.149e-05 −1.278e-05
HaHa −1.417e-06 4.034e-05*** −1.119e-04*** −4.273e-05*** −5.032e-04*** −4.743e-04***
Wow −7.652e-05** −1.114e-05 9.149e-06 −5.246e-05 −3.544e-04*** −6.379e-06
Sad −6.757e-06 5.480e-06 5.575e-05*** −2.893e-05 5.854e-05*** 4.601e-05***
Angry −2.256e-05 2.704e-05 −7.572e-06 −1.995e-04*** 6.300e-05*** 5.317e-05***
Note:

Significance codes: ***p < 0.001;

**p < 0.01;

*p < 0.05

Regression results on the relationship between the centrality measures and emotions

Independent variables Dependent variable
Betweenness Degree Eigenvalue
(H14)
Love 6.131e-06 *** 5.429e-06 *** 7.051e-06
HaHa −1.737e-06 ** 3.250e-05 *** 3.344e-05
Wow −1.009e-04 *** −1.956e-05 *** −4.618e-05
Sad 9.083e-06 *** −1.141e-05 *** −2.515e-05
Angry 2.025e-05 *** 2.279e-05 *** 2.535e-05
(H15)
p.Anger 0.019 *** −0.023 *** −0.026
p.Anticipation 0.098 *** 0.021 *** −0.009
p.Disgust −0.018 *** 0.018 *** 0.043
p.Fear 0.008 *** 0.008 *** −0.018
p.Joy −0.167 *** −0.095 *** −0.129 ***
p.Sadness 0.024 *** −0.041 *** −0.058
p.Surprise 0.151 *** 0.139 *** 0.225 ***
p.Trust 0.132 *** 0.087 *** 0.122 ***
p.Negative 0.088 *** 0.023 *** 0.022
p.Positive 0.127 *** 0.067 *** 0.075 ***
Note:

Significance codes: ***p < 0.001;

**p < 0.01;

*p < 0.05

Factorization of the post variables

Factor In pages p.Anticipation p.Anger p.Disgust p.Fear p.Joy p.Sadness p.Surprise p.Trust p.Positive p.Negative
p1 7
p2 9
p3 1
p4 1

Factorization of the post and comment variables

Factors (page)
Variable pc1 (BBC) pc2 (Fox) pc3 (NYT) pc4 (BBC) pc5 (Fox) pc6 (Huffington) pc7 (Washington) pc8 (Yahoo) pc9 (Guardian)
p.Anticipation
p.Anger
p.Disgust
p.Fear
p.Joy
p.Sadness
p.Surprise
p.Trust
p.Positive
p.Negative
c.Anicipation
c.Anger
c.Disgust
c.Fear
c.Joy
c.Sadness
c.Surprise
c.Trust
c.Positive
c.Negative

Factorization of the comment variables

Factor Page c.Anticipation c.Anger c.Disgust c.Fear c.Joy c.Sadness c.Surprise c.Trust c.Positive c.Negative
c1 CNN
c2 LAT, Wallstreet
c3 NBC
c4 CBS
c5 ABC
c6 Yahoo
c7 Washington
c8 USA

Factorization of the network variables

Factor Page n.Betweenness n.Closeness n.Degree n.Eigenvalue p.Trust p.Surprise p.Positive c.Negative
n1 CNN, Fox, Guardian, NBC, NPR, NYT, USA, Yahoo
n2 Huffington, LAT
n3 ABC, BBC
n4 CBS
n5 Wallstreet
n6 Washington

References

Acheampong, F.A., Wenyu, C. and Nunoo-Mensah, H. (2020), “Text-based emotion detection: advances, challenges, and opportunities”, Engineering Reports, Vol. 2 No. 7, p. e12189.

Aldous, K.K., An, J. and Jansen, B.J. (2021), “Measuring 9 emotions of news posts from 8 news organizations across 4 social media platforms for 8 months”, ACM Transactions on Social Computing, Article 15 (December 2021), Vol. 4 No. 4, p. 31.

Almgren, S.M. and Olsson, T. (2016), “Commenting, sharing and tweeting news measuring online news participation”, Nordicom Review, Vol. 37 No. 2, pp. 67-81.

Al-Rawi, A. (2017), “News values on social media: news organizations Facebook use”, Journalism, Vol. 18 No. 7, pp. 871-889.

Al-Zaman, M.S. and Ahona, T.A. (2022), “Users’ reactions to rape news shared on social media: an analysis of five Facebook reaction buttons”, Asian Journal for Public Opinion Research, Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 51-73, doi: 10.15206/ajpor.2022.10.1.51.

Avdić, D. and Bagić Babac, M. (2021), “Application of affective lexicons in sports text mining: a case study of FIFA world cup 2018”, South Eastern European Journal of Communication, Vol. 3 No. 2, pp. 23-33.

Banerjee, S. and Dutta, U. (2015), “Detection of emotions in text: a survey”, International Journal of Advanced Engineering and Global Technology, Vol. 3 No. 12, pp. 1436-1439.

Calderon, F.H. and Kuo, P.C. (2019), “Emotion combination in social media comments as features for sarcasm detection”, WISDOM ‘19, August 04, 2019, Anchorage, AK.

Canales, L. and Martinez-Barco, P. (2014), “Emotion detection from text: a survey”, Proceedings of the Workshop on Natural Language Processing in the 5th Information Systems Research Working Days, Faculty of Systems Engineering. National Polytechnic School of Ecuador, Quito, Ecuador, pp. 37-43.

Choi, J., Yup, S., Sung, L. and Ji, W. (2020), “Engagement in emotional news on social media: intensity and type of emotions”, Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, Vol. 98 No. 4.

Clos, J., Bandhakavi, A., Wiratunga, N. and Cabanac, G. (2017), “Predicting emotional reaction in social networks”, 39th European Colloquium on Information Retrieval (ECIR 2017), Apr 2017, Aberdeen, ffhal-01809392, pp. 527-533.

Cvitanović, I. and Bagić Babac, M. (2022), “Deep learning with self-attention mechanism for fake news detection”, Combating Fake News with Computational Intelligence Techniques/Lahby M.; Pathan as.K.; Maleh Y.; Yafooz W.M.S. (ur.), Springer, Switzerland, pp. 205-229, doi:10.1007/978-3-030-90087-8_10.

Dolan, R., Conduit, J., Fahy, J. and Goodman, S. (2015), “Social media engagement behaviour: a uses and gratifications perspective”, Journal of Strategic Marketing, Vol. 24 Nos 3/4, pp. 261-277.

Dwivedi, A., Johnson, L.W., Wilkie, D.C. and De Araujo-Gil, L. (2019), “Consumer emotional brand attachment with social media brands and social media brand equity”, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 53 No. 6, pp. 1176-1204.

Ekman, P. (1992), “An argument for basic emotions”, Cognition and Emotion, Vol. 6 Nos 3/4, pp. 169-200.

Festinger, L. (1962), “Cognitive dissonance”, Scientific American, Vol. 207 No. 4, pp. 93-107.

Gambino, O.J., Calvo, H. and García-Mendoza, C.-V. (2018), “Distribution of emotional reactions to news articles in twitter”, Proceedings of the Eleventh International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC-2018), European Language Resource Association.

Glader, P. (2017), “10 Journalism brands where you find real facts rather than alternative facts”, Forbes, 1 Feb., available at: www.forbes.com/sites/berlinschoolofcreativeleadership/2017/02/01/10-journalism-brands-where-you-will-find-real-facts-rather-than-alternative-facts/ (accessed 20 December 2018).

Guerra, P.C., Meira, W.J. and Cardie, C. (2014), “Sentiment analysis on evolving social streams: how self-report imbalances can help”, WSDM’14, ACM, New York, NY.

Gummerus, J., Liljander, V., Weman, E. and Pihlström, M. (2012), “Customer engagement in a Facebook brand community”, Management Research Review, Vol. 35 No. 9, pp. 857-877.

Hamari, J. and Sjöblom, M. (2017), “What is eSports and why do people watch it?”, Internet Research, Vol. 27 No. 2, pp. 211-232.

Hampton, K., Goulet, L.S., Rainie, L. and Purcell, K. (2011), “Social networking sites and our lives”, Report, Pew Internet & American Life Project.

Han, M.C. (2021), “Thumbs down on ‘likes’? The impact of Facebook reactions on online consumers' nonprofit engagement behavior”, International Review on Public and Nonprofit Marketing, Vol. 18 No. 2, pp. 255-272.

Harmon-Jones, E. (2000), “A cognitive dissonance theory perspective on the role of emotion in the maintenance and change of beliefs and attitudes”, in Frijda, N., Manstead, A. and Bem, S. (Eds), Emotions and Beliefs: How Feelings Influence Thoughts (Studies in Emotion and Social Interaction, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 185-211.

Hochschild, A. (1983), The Managed Heart: Commercialization of Human Feeling, University of CA Press, Berkeley, CA.

Jackson, M.O. (2008), Social and Economic Networks, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.

Jansz, J. and Timmers, M. (2002), “Emotional dissonance: when the experience of an emotion jeopardizes an individual’s identity”, Theory & Psychology, Vol. 12 No. 1, pp. 79-95.

Jurafsky, D. and Martin, J.H. (2015), Speech and Language Processing, Chapter 21: Lexicons for Sentiment and Affect Extraction.

Kampić, M. and Bagić Babac, M. (2021), “Sentiment analysis of president trump’s tweets: from winning the election to the fight against COVID-19”, Communication Management Review, Vol. 6 No. 2, pp. 90-111, doi: 10.22522/cmr20210272.

Katz, E. and Lazarsfeld, P.F. (1955), Personal Influence: The Part Played by People in the Flow of Mass Communication, Free Press, Glencoe, IL.

Katz, E., Blumler, J.G. and Gurevitch, M. (1973), “Uses and gratifications research”, Public Opinion Quarterly, Vol. 37 No. 4, pp. 509-23.

Katz, E., Haas, H. and Gurevitch, M. (1974), “On the use of the mass media for important things”, American Sociological Review, Vol. 38 No. 2, pp. 164-181.

Kawade, S. and Waghmare, K.C. (2017), “A survey on identification of emotion from text corpus”, International Journal of Innovative Research in Computer and Communication Engineering, Vol. 5 No. 3, pp. 3882-3886.

Khobzi, H., Lau, R.Y.K. and Cheung, T.C.H. (2019), “The outcome of online social interactions on facebook pages: a study of user engagement behavior”, Internet Research, Vol. 29 No. 1, pp. 2-23.

Kramer, A.D., Guillory, J.E. and Hancock, J.T. (2014), “Experimental evidence of massive-scale emotional contagion through social networks”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Vol. 111 No. 24, pp. 8788-8790.

Krebs, F., Lubascher, B., Moers, T., Schaap, P. and Spanakis, G. (2018), “Social emotion mining techniques for Facebook posts reaction prediction”, Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Agents and Artificial Intelligence (ICAART 2018), Vol. 2, pp. 211-220.

Kümpel, A.S., Karnowski, V. and Keyling, T. (2015), “News sharing in social media: a review of current research on news sharing users, content, and networks”, Social Media + Society, Vol. 1 No. 2.

Leung, L. (2013), “Generational differences in content generation in social media: the roles of the gratifications sought and of narcissism”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 29 No. 3, pp. 997-1006.

Li, X., Peng, Q., Sun, Z., Chai, L. and Wang, Y. (2017), “Predicting social emotions from readers’ perspective”, IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, Vol. 10 No. 2.

Lin, K.H.-Y., Yang, C. and Chen, H.-H. (2008), “Emotion classification of online news articles from the reader’s perspective”, International Conference on Web Intelligence and Intelligent Agent Technology, IEEE, Vol. 1, pp. 220-226.

Luarn, P., Lin, Y.-F. and Chiu, Y.-P. (2015), “Influence of Facebook brand-page posts on online engagement”, Online Information Review, Vol. 39 No. 4, pp. 505-519.

Malthouse, E.C., Haenlein, M., Skiera, B., Wege, E. and Zhang, M. (2013), “Managing customer relationships in the social media era: introducing the social CRM house”, Journal of Interactive Marketing, Vol. 27 No. 4, pp. 270-280.

Mohammad, S.M. and Turney, P. (2010), “Emotions evoked by common words and phrases: using mechanical Turk to create an emotion lexicon”, Proceedings of the NAACL-HLT 2010 Workshop on Computational Approaches to Analysis and Generation of Emotion in Text, LA, CA.

Mohammad, S.M. and Turney, P. (2013), “Crowdsourcing a word-emotion association lexicon”, Computational Intelligence, Vol. 29 No. 3, pp. 436-465.

Molina, A., Gomez, M., Lyon, A., Aranda, E. and Loibl, W. (2020), “What content to post? Evaluating the effectiveness of Facebook communications in destinations”, Journal of Destination Marketing & Management, Vol. 18 No. 100498, doi: 10.1016/j.jdmm.2020.100498.

Moussa, S. (2019), “An emoji-based metric for monitoring consumers’ emotions toward brands on social media”, Marketing Intelligence & Planning, Vol. 37 No. 2, pp. 211-225, doi: 10.1108/MIP-07-2018-025.

Murthy, A.R. and Anil Kumar, K.M. (2021), “A review of different approaches for detecting emotion from text”, IOP Conference Series: Materials Science and Engineering, Vol. 1110 No. 1, p. 012009.

Plutchik, R. (1962), The Emotions: Facts, Theories, and a New Model, Random House.

Preston, S., Anderson, A., Robertson, D.J., Shephard, M.P. and Huhe, N. (2021), “Detecting fake news on Facebook: the role of emotional intelligence”, Plos One, Vol. 16 No. 10, p. e0258719.

Puh, K. and Bagić Babac, M. (2022), “Predicting sentiment and rating of tourist reviews using machine learning”, Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Insights, (in press), doi: 10.1108/JHTI-02-2022-0078.

Rao, Y. (2016), “Contextual sentiment topic model for adaptive social emotion classification”, IEEE Intelligent Systems, Vol. 31 No. 1, pp. 41-47.

Rimé, B., Mesquita, B., Boca, S. and Philippot, P. (1991), “Beyond the emotional event: six studies on the social sharing of emotion”, Cognition & Emotion, Vol. 5 Nos 5/6, pp. 435-465.

Russell, J.A. (1980), “A circumplex model of affect”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 39 No. 6, p. 1161.

Sandoval-Almazan, R. and Valle-Cruz, D. (2020), “Sentiment analysis of Facebook users reacting to political campaign posts”, Digital Government: Research and Practice, Article 12 (April 2020), Vol. 1 No. 2, p. 13.

Scherer, K.R. (2000), “Psychological models of emotion”, in Borod, J.C. (Ed.), The Neuropsychology of Emotion, Oxford, pp. 137-162.

Severin, W. and Tankard, J. (1997), Communication Theories: Origins, Methods, and Uses in the Mass Media, Longman, New York.

Shen, J., Najand, M., Dong, F. and He, W. (2017), “News and social media emotions in the commodity market”, Review of Behavioral Finance, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 148-168.

Silge, J. and Robinson, D. (2018), Text Mining with R, O'Reilly, Sebastopol, USA.

Tian, Y., Galery, T., Dulcinati, G., Molimpakis, E. and Sun, C. (2017), “Facebook sentiment: reactions and emojis”, Proceedings of the Fifth International Workshop on Natural Language Processing for Social Media, pp. 11-16.

Tomkins, S.S. (1962), “Affect, imagery, consciousness”, Vol. I, The Positive Affects, Springer, New York.

Tripathi, V., Joshi, A. and Bhattacharyya, P. (2016), “Emotion analysis from text: a survey”, Center for Indian Language Technology Surveys, Vol. 11 No. 8, pp. 66-69.

Tzacheva, A., Ranganathan, J. and Mylavarapu, S.Y. (2019), “Actionable pattern discovery for tweet emotions”, Proceedings of the International Conference on Applied Human Factors and Ergonomics, Springer, pp. 46-57.

Verto Analytics (2018), “Most popular news brands in the United States as of June 2018, by number of PC users (in millions)”, Statista, available at: www.statista.com/statistics/875894/most-popular-us-news-brands-number-pc-users/ (accessed 1 February 2019).

Woodruff, S.J., Coyne, P., Fulcher, J., Reagan, R., Rowdon, L., Santarossa, S. and Pegoraro, A. (2020), “Reaction on social media to online news headlines following the release of Canada's food guide”, Canadian Journal of Dietetic Practice and Research, Vol. 82 No. 1, pp. 16-20.

Zech, E., Rimé, B. and Nils, F. (2004), “Social sharing of emotion, emotional recovery, and interpersonal aspects”, in Philippot, P. and Feldman, R.S. (Eds), The Regulation of Emotion, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, pp. 157-185.

Further reading

Bagić Babac, M. and Podobnik, V. (2016), “A sentiment analysis of who participates, how and why, at social media sport websites: how differently men and women write about football”, Online Information Review, Vol. 40 No. 6, pp. 814-833.

Bandhakavi, A., Wiratunga, N., Massie, S. and Padmanabhan, D. (2017), “Lexicon generation for emotion detection from text”, IEEE Intelligent Systems, Vol. 32 No. 1, pp. 102-108.

Berger, J. and Milkman, K.L. (2012), “What makes online content viral?”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 49 No. 2, pp. 192-205.

Eberl, J.M., Tolochko, P., Jost, P., Heidenreich, T. and Boomgaarden, H.G. (2020), “What's in a post? How sentiment and issue salience affect users' emotional reactions on facebook”, Journal of Information Technology & Politics, Vol. 17 No. 1, pp. 48-65.

Freeman, C., Alhoori, H. and Shahzad, M. (2020), “Measuring the diversity of Facebook reactions to research”, Proceedings of the ACM on Human-Computer Interaction, Vol. 4 No. GROUP, pp. 1-17.

Freeman, C., Roy, M.K., Fattoruso, M. and Alhoori, H. (2019), “Shared feelings: understanding Facebook reactions to scholarly articles”, Proceedings of the ACM/IEEE Joint Conference on Digital Libraries, 2019-June, pp. 301-304.

Geboers, M., Stolero, N., Scuttari, A., Van Vliet, L. and Ridley, A. (2020), “Why buttons matter: repurposing Facebook's reactions for analysis of the social visual”, International Journal of Communication, Vol. 14, pp. 1564-1585.

Giuntini, F.T., Ruiz, L.P., Kirchner, L.D.F., Passarelli, D.A., Dos Reis, M.D.J.D., Campbell, A.T. and Ueyama, J. (2019), “How do I feel? Identifying emotional expressions on Facebook reactions using clustering mechanism”, IEEE Access, Vol. 7, pp. 53909-53921, doi: 10.1109/ACCESS.2019.2913136.

Guerini, M. and Staiano, J. (2015), “Deep feelings: a massive cross-lingual study on the relation between emotions and virality”, Proceedings of the 24th International Conference on World Wide Web, ACM, Florence, Italy, pp. 299-305.

Hou, J.-R. and Kankham, S. (2022), “More than feelings? How Facebook reaction icons affect online users' behavioral intentions toward online health rumor posts”, Internet Research, Vol. 32 No. 6, pp. 1978-2002, doi: 10.1108/INTR-04-2021-0236

Jiang, D., Luo, X., Xuan, J. and Xu, Z. (2017), “Sentiment computing for the news event based on the big social media data”, IEEE Access, Vol. 5, pp. 2373-2382.

Jost, P., Maurer, M. and Hassler, J. (2020), “Populism fuels love and anger: the impact of message features on users’ reactions on Facebook”, International Journal of Communication, Vol. 14, pp. 2081-2102.

Larsson, A.O. (2018), “Diversifying likes”, Journalism Practice, Vol. 12 No. 3, pp. 326-343.

Li, X., Rao, Y., Chen, Y., Liu, X. and Huang, H. (2016), “Social emotion classification via reader perspective weighted model”, Proceedings of the Thirtieth AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence (AAAI-16), pp. 4230-4231.

Medhat, W., Hassan, A. and Korashy, H. (2014), “Sentiment analysis algorithms and applications: a survey”, Ain Shams Engineering Journal, Vol. 5 No. 4, pp. 1093-1113.

Mohammad, S., Dunne, C. and Dorr, B. (2009), “Generating high-coverage semantic orientation lexicons from overly marked words and a thesaurus”, Proceedings of the Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing, pp. 599-608.

Preacher, K.J. and MacCallum, R.C. (2003), “Repairing Tom Swift's electric factor analysis machine”, Understanding Statistics, Vol. 2 No. 1, pp. 13-43.

Smoliarova, A.S., Gromova, T.M. and Pavlushkina, N.A. (2018), “Emotional stimuli in social media user behavior: emoji reactions on a news media Facebook page”, Lecture Notes in Computer Science (Including Subseries Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence and Lecture Notes in Bioinformatics), Vol. 11193, pp. 242-256, doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-01437-7_19.

Wilkerson, H.S., Riedl, M.J. and Whipple, K.N. (2021), “Affective affordances: exploring Facebook reactions as emotional responses to hyperpartisan political news”, Digital Journalism, Vol. 9 No. 8, pp. 1040-1061.

Corresponding author

Marina Bagić Babac can be contacted at: marina.bagic@fer.hr

Related articles