Gender differences in mental health prevalence in autism

Felicity Sedgewick (Department of Education, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK and Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK)
Jenni Leppanen (Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK)
Kate Tchanturia (Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry Psychology and Neuroscience, South London and Maudsley Mental Health NHS Trust, London, UK, and Ilia State University, Tbilisi, Georgia)

Advances in Autism

ISSN: 2056-3868

Article publication date: 6 June 2020

Issue publication date: 11 August 2021

2167

Abstract

Purpose

Mental health conditions are known to be more common amongst autistic than non-autistic people. To date, there is little work exploring gender differences in mental health amongst autistic people and no work including non-binary/trans people. This paper aims to address this gap.

Design/methodology/approach

This was a large-scale online study, with 948 participants between 18 and 81 years old. Participants self-reported autism, anxiety, depression and eating disorder status. Analyses were run examining gender differences in the rates of these conditions in each group.

Findings

Autistic people are more likely to have anxiety and depression than non-autistic people of all genders. Autistic women and non-binary people experienced mental health issues at higher rates than men and at similar rates to each other. Autistic people were twice as likely as non-autistic people to have all eating disorders. Further, gendered patterns of eating disorders seen in the non-autistic population are also present in the autistic population.

Research limitations/implications

There are inherent issues with self-report of diagnoses online, but this study showed that using screening questionnaires is effective.

Originality/value

This is the first paper to look at gender differences in common mental health issues amongst autistic and non-autistic adults. It highlights that there are significant gendered patterns in the prevalence of mental health issues in both the autistic and non-autistic population and that these have an impact for how treatment should be approached to be effective.

Keywords

Citation

Sedgewick, F., Leppanen, J. and Tchanturia, K. (2021), "Gender differences in mental health prevalence in autism", Advances in Autism, Vol. 7 No. 3, pp. 208-224. https://doi.org/10.1108/AIA-01-2020-0007

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2020, Emerald Publishing Limited


Approximately 1%−2% of the general population are diagnosed as autistic, with current gender ratios standing at 3:1 male:female (Loomes et al., 2017). This ratio has decreased from earlier estimates of 12:1 male:female (Fombonne, 2003) largely due to increased recognition of girls and women as being on the autism spectrum (Gould and Ashton-Smith, 2011; Haney, 2016; Lai et al., 2015). Being autistic is a minority identity in a non-autistic majority world, and those who are non-male also face the experience of a minority identity in a society which is generally patriarchal (Miller, 2013). This means that autistic non-men are potentially subject to greater stressors, leading to higher rates of mental health issues, according to multiple minority theory (Meyer, 1995; Botha and Frost, 2018).

There are a range of mental health conditions, which occur in the general population – with anxiety and depression being most common, affecting between 3.8% and 25% of adults (Brody et al., 2013; Remes et al., 2016). Eating disorders (ED), although occurring at lower rates (approximately 1%−6% of the population) (Smink et al., 2012; Micali et al., 2013) are also relatively common, and are approximately ten times more commonly diagnosed in women than men (Raevuori et al., 2014). EDs have a high level of co-occurrence with anxiety and/or depression, ranging from 9% to 71% of ED patients having a major mood or anxiety disorder in different studies (Godart et al., 2007; Swinbourne and Touyz, 2007), and so any study of ED rates should also investigate these conditions.

It has long been recognised that autistic people are more likely to experience a range of mental health issues than non-autistic people (White et al., 2009). Anxiety and depression are the most common mental health issues for those on the spectrum, as they are for non-autistic people, with up to 40% of autistic people having at least one anxiety disorder (van Steensel et al., 2011) and similar numbers having clinical depression (Kim et al., 2000). Difficulties around food also appear to be more common in autistic children than their non-autistic counterparts (Schreck et al., 2004), such as food selectivity and refusal (Cermak et al., 2010). While a range of underlying causes for these difficulties have been suggested, such as sensory sensitivities (Chistol et al., 2018; Dovey et al., 2019), the eating behaviours of autistic adults have not been investigated in the same way. One review commented on the relative paucity of population-based research into EDs amongst autistic adults but found a number of case studies reporting unusual eating behaviour, including pica, rumination and food refusal, to be common (Rastam, 2008). The only qualitative study of eating behaviour amongst autistic adults found that childhood behaviours continued into adulthood, especially in the areas of sensory sensitivity, medical or physical issues, and the impact of executive function challenges on eating, but that adults felt they had mostly developed strategies for managing these eating behaviours (Kinnaird et al., 2019).

A link between autism and anorexia nervosa (AN) has long been theorised (Gillberg, 1985). Research has shown that up to 23% of women with AN meet clinical criterion for an autism diagnosis (Westwood et al., 2017), and other works have shown significant overlap between the cognitive profiles of the two conditions (Westwood and Tchanturia, 2017). There has been speculation that women with severe and treatment resistant AN may have underlying autism which has gone unrecognised (Oldershaw et al., 2011), partly because there is a clinical focus on treating the immediate danger of AN rather than consideration of potential underlying autism, a condition more frequently seen in men (Gould, 2017) which therefore may not spring to mind in a female AN patient. However, previous research examining this potential shared aetiology has primarily investigated the presence of autistic traits amongst people with AN with a focus on women.

To date, however, there has been little work exploring whether there are gender differences in rates of mental health conditions within the autistic population. What work there is has focussed on a binary conception of gender – male and female (Worley and Matson, 2011; May et al., 2014; Magiati et al., 2016). There is growing evidence that autistic adults are more likely to be non-binary and non-heteronormative (Dewinter et al., 2017), in terms of their gender identity. This means that they likely face unique life experiences and potentially are exposed to even higher rates of discrimination and difficulty than cisgender autistic people, following the “multiple minority theory” (Meyer, 1995). Multiple minority theory argues that those with a minority identity, such as being homosexual, experience more everyday stressors than those who are in the majority and that these stressors are multiplied if you are a member of multiple minority groups, such as being both gay and black. Within this approach, being autistic can be seen as a form of minority identity, and so can being female, non-binary, trans or LGBT+. However, in line with an update to this theory, these individuals are also potentially more resilient than those in the majority, as they have both the autism community and the LGBT+ community to draw on for support (Meyer, 2015). Despite this, there are known health inequalities amongst non-autistic non-binary people (Whitehead, 2017; Jones et al., 2019) which are likely to also affect autistic people, who report extensive difficulties in accessing mental health support (Camm-Crosbie et al., 2019; Crane et al., 2019). It is therefore important to investigate the intersection of these minority identities regarding mental health.

This study sought to examine the rates at which anxiety, depression and a range of EDs are reported amongst a large sample of autistic and non-autistic adults of all genders. Our hypotheses were:

H1.

Autistic people of all genders would report higher levels of anxiety and depression symptomatology than non-autistic people

H2.

Women and non-binary/trans people would report anxiety, depression and all types of ED at higher rates than men, regardless of autism status.

H3.

There would be positive correlations between autistic traits and anxiety, depression and ED symptomatology across both groups.

Method

Participants

It is observed that 945 people between 18 and 81 years old were included in the analysis, after the exclusion of 14 participants who reported being under 18 (being aged 18+ was an inclusion criteria) and 18 participants who reported being non-autistic but scored above cut-off on the AQ-28. Of these 945, 525 (55.50%) reported being autistic and 420 (44.50%) reported no autism diagnosis. Non-binary and trans participants were combined into one NBT group for statistical validity, as otherwise, the numbers involved in each group would have been too small to meaningfully include. Please see Table 1 for demographic information about each group.

Participants were recruited online through social media (Twitter, Facebook) and advertising on the King’s College website and email circulars and were offered the chance to take part in a draw for one of 30 £10 Amazon vouchers as an incentive. The study was open between December 2017 and March 2018. Participants completed the study online, hosted on Qualtrics, at their own pace and in a place of their preference. An information sheet and consent form was the first page of the online questionnaire, detailing the content of the study, the expected length of time to complete and participant rights such as the right to withdraw. The data was collected as part of a larger study, for which ethical approval was obtained from the King’s Psychiatry, Nursing and Midwifery Research Ethics Committee (LRS-17/18–5292).

Measures

  • Demographics: Participants completed a demographics questionnaire, including age, self-defined gender, height, weight, ethnicity, education level and employment status. Participants were also asked whether they had a diagnosis of autism, any physical or mental health diagnosis and/or a diagnosed ED, and if so, what that diagnosis was.

  • AQ: The Autism Quotient-28 item version (Hoekstra et al., 2011) is a self-report screening questionnaire assessing the presence and level of autism symptomatology. Answers are given on a Likert scale from “very accurate” to “very inaccurate” and are then scored 1 or 0 depending on the direction of the question. Higher scores reflect more autistic symptomatology. The abbreviated version of this measure was used in this study to reduce participant burden, but it still has a high level of reliability (α = 0.77 − 0.86=).

  • HADS: The hospital anxiety and depression scale (Zigmond and Snaith, 1983) is a 14-item self-report questionnaire assessing both anxiety and depression symptomatology. The HADS creates two subscales, HADS-Anxiety (HADS-A) and HADS-Depression (HADS-D), each with seven items. It has been widely used in a variety of populations and has been validated for use with autistic people (Uljarević et al., 2018). Higher scores reflect higher levels of anxiety and depression symptomatology.

  • EDE-Q: The eating disorder examination self-report questionnaire (Fairburn and Beglin, 1994) is a 36-item self-report questionnaire assessing ED psychopathology over the past 28 days. Participants score the frequency of their behaviours or thoughts from “0 days” to “every day”. Higher scores reflect greater ED symptomatology.

Data analysis

All data analyses were conducted with R (R Core Team, 2018). Group differences in demographic and clinical characteristics were explored with t-tests. Prior to conducting regression analyses, assumptions were checked. Where assumptions for a linear regression analyses were not met, impact of gender and autism status on ED, anxiety and depression symptomatology were investigated using robust multiple regression from the MASS package (rlm()) (Venables and Ripley, 2002). The rlm() function conducts a robust M-estimator with Huber’s weights to reduce the impact of outliers and heteroscedasticity (Huber and Ronchetti, 1981). Where significant main effects or interactions were present, post hoc pairwise comparisons were conducted. All statistics from post hoc tests were adjusted for multiple comparisons using Boferroni correction. Differences between the autistic and non-autistic groups in the number of participants reporting a diagnosis of an eating disorder were investigated using robust binomial regression from the robustbase package (glmrob()) (Maechler et al., 2019). Correlations between AQ-28 score and ED, anxiety and depression symptomatology were explored within the autistic and non-autistic groups using Spearman test. Significance level was set at p < 0.05.

Results

Demographics

Participants were not matched on age, t(911) = −2.38, p = 0.02, with autistic participants being older than non-autistic. Participants were also not matched on AQ score, with those who reported being autistic scoring significantly higher than those who reported being non-autistic, t(680) = −34.02, p < 0.001, supporting their self-reported diagnoses. People who reported having anxiety, depression and a current or past ED scored significantly higher on the relevant questionnaires, supporting their self-reported diagnoses (all p < 0.01). Demographic characteristics of the sample can be seen in Table 1.

Anxiety

The data did not meet the assumptions for linear multiple regression (Figure A1) and robust regression was conducted. The robust multiple regression revealed a significant effect of autism status [Table 2; Figure 1(a)] such that autistic participants reported significantly more anxiety symptoms on the HADS than the non-autistic participants. There was also a significant effect of gender. Post hoc pairwise comparisons showed that female participants reported experiencing significantly more anxiety than male participants regardless of autism status (Z = 3.30, p = 0.003). Male participants also reported significantly less anxiety than NBT participants regardless of autism status (Z = −3.25, p = 0.003). There was no significant difference between female and NBT participants in anxiety (Z = −1.32, p = 0.566). There was no significant autism status by gender interaction.

Correlation analyses found a significant positive correlation between HADS anxiety and AQ-28 scores amongst both the autistic (ρ = 0.16, p < 0.001) and non-autistic participants (ρ = 0.40, p < 0.001) [Figure 2(a) and 2(b)].

Depression

The data did not meet the assumptions for linear multiple regression (Figure A2) and robust regression was conducted. The robust multiple regression revealed a significant effect of autism status [Table 2; Figure 1(b)]. Autistic participants reported significantly more depression symptoms on the HADS than non-autistic participants. There was also a significant interaction between autism status and gender. Post hoc pairwise comparisons showed that non-autistic women and men reported significantly less depression that non-autistic NBT participants (Z = −3.08, p = 0.006 and Z = −2.59, p = 0.029, respectively). There were no significant differences in depression scores between non-autistic female and non-autistic male participants (Z = −0.27, p = 1.00). There were also no significant differences between autistic female and autistic male participants (Z = 0.76, p = 1.00), autistic female and autistic NBT participants (Z = 1.59, p = 0.335) or autistic male and autistic NBT participants (Z = 0.39, p = 1.00).

Correlations analysis found a significant positive relationship between HADS depression and AQ-28 scores in both autistic (ρ = 0.21, p < 0.001) and non-autistic groups (ρ = 0.48, p < 0.001) [Figure 2(c) and 2(d)].

Eating disorders

The data did not meet the assumptions for linear multiple regression (Figure A3) and robust regression was conducted. The robust multiple regression revealed a significant effect of autism status [Table 2; Figure 1(c)]. Autistic participants reported significantly more ED symptoms on the EDEQ than non-autistic participants. There was also a significant effect of gender. Post hoc pairwise comparisons showed that female participants reported significantly more ED symptoms than male participants (Z = 3.54, p = 0.001) regardless of autism status. There was no significant difference between female and NBT participants (Z = 1.96, p = 0.152) or male and NBT participants (Z = −0.65, p = 1.00) regardless of autism status. There was no significant autism status by gender interaction.

As the above finding may have been influenced by the number of participants reporting having been diagnosed with an ED further analyses were conducted. Indeed, a robust binomial regression showed that significantly more autistic participants reported having been diagnosed with an ED than non-autistic participants (Table 2). Therefore, an additional robust regression analysis was conducted amongst participants who did not report having an ED to explore if the difference between autistic and non-autistic groups was due to over-representation of ED symptomatology amongst the autistic participants or was driven by difference in number of participants reporting ED diagnosis. The robust regression again revealed a significant effect of gender (F(2) = 3.87, p = 0.021). However, post hoc pairwise comparisons showed that the difference between female and male participants only approached significance (Z = 2.10, p = 0.091). There were no significant differences between female and NBT participants (Z = 1.84, p = 0.156) or male and NBT participants (Z = 0.40, p = 0.915) who did not report an ED diagnosis. The previous finding of significant difference between autistic and non-autistic participants in ED symptomatology was not replicated amongst participants who did not report having an ED (F(1) = 2.40, p = 0.122). There was also no significant autism status by gender interaction (F(2) = 0.15, p = 0.861).

Correlation analyses found significant positive relationships between EDEQ and AQ-28 scores amongst both autistic (ρ = 0.11, p = 0.013) and non-autistic participants (ρ = 0.13, p = 0.007) [Figure 2(e) and 2(f)].

Discussion

This study investigated the prevalence of self-reported anxiety, depression and ED amongst autistic and non-autistic people of all genders. Our findings suggest autistic people are more likely to have anxiety and depression than their non-autistic counterparts, and that as autistic symptomatology increases so do mental health issues. While autistic people were more likely to have an existing ED diagnosis, we also found that as autistic traits rose, so did ED symptomatology. Importantly, we found that gender plays a role in the levels of anxiety, depression and eating disorder behaviours which participants endorsed, emphasising that it is not just being autistic which has an impact on mental health. This is evidence for the applicability of multiple minority theory in the context of autism research and lived experience.

Anxiety

As expected according to H1, and in line with a wealth of previous research, autistic people of all genders were more anxious than their non-autistic counterparts. This fits with a pattern of findings showing that autistic people experience high levels of anxiety across the lifespan, from childhood (Gillott et al., 2001) through adolescence (Kuusikko et al., 2008; Magiati et al., 2016) and into adulthood (Bejerot et al., 2014; Maddox and White, 2015).

What was novel, and was in line with H2, was the finding of a gendered pattern to these experiences of anxiety. While all autistic groups were more anxious than non-autistic groups, women and NBT people were significantly more anxious than men, regardless of autism status. This supports the idea that multiple minority theory (Meyer, 1995) can be meaningfully applied to the experiences of autistic people, with a notable impact of non-male gender on anxiety. It may be that being autistic, combined with belonging to groups who are traditionally under-valued in a patriarchal and heteronormative society, leads to more daily stressors than being autistic and male does. Daily stressors can be things such as the high levels of stigma and bullying autistic people are subjected to (Rowley et al., 2012) or the difficulties sensory sensitivities can present in navigating the world (Gillott and Standen, 2007). The impact of being a multiple minority – autistic and non-male – can be seen in the difficulties autistic women have. For example, they suffer from being judged as not only meeting traditional female stereotypes (Baldwin and Costley, 2016; Kanfiszer et al., 2017) but also struggle to access an autism diagnosis because they do not fit a “male stereotype” (Gould, 2017; Gould and Ashton-Smith, 2011). The challenges facing autistic NBT people in both daily life and in accessing autism diagnoses and support have not yet been investigated but are likely to be similar, as they also likely do not meet the male stereotype of autism and are statistically a gender-identity minority.

Following H3, the correlation results suggested that increased anxiety is related to higher levels of autistic traits also suggests that elements such as social difficulty or sensory sensitivities have an impact on the mental health of everyone. It may be that finding communities where people feel supported in their identity, whether that is around their gender or autism status, can help to minimise the levels of anxiety they experience. Indeed, there is growing evidence that autistic people socialise better together than they do with non-autistic people (Crompton, 2019; Crompton et al., 2019; Heasman and Gillespie, 2018). This provides hope that the autism community may provide the same protective effects as have been seen in the LGBT+ community (Roberts et al., 2011; Budge et al., 2014), although this support cannot entirely counteract the stressors presented by the wider world in general.

Depression

Regarding depression, and consistent with H1, it was unsurprising to find that autistic people scored more highly than their non-autistic counterparts, regardless of gender. For example, a recent study has shown that autistic people are more likely than non-autistic people to have been unemployed, homeless and suffered domestic abuse (Griffiths et al., 2019). Experiencing more negative life events may naturally lead to a higher likelihood of developing depression.

Counter to the prediction of H2, the lack of gender differences on depression amongst the autistic sample is in stark contrast to the anxiety findings. This may be because while a greater number of stressors make autistic women and NBT people more anxious, whether this anxiety is paired with low mood in response to these stressors is more individual and so there are fewer clear group-level effects. However, growing research on self-harm and suicide amongst autistic people finds that autistic women (autistic NBT people are not included in these studies) are more likely to attempt and complete suicide than autistic men (Cassidy and Rodgers, 2017). Camouflaging (consciously attempting to reduce visible autistic traits), a phenomenon known to be more common amongst autistic women (Hull et al., 2019), has been found to be a specific risk factor for suicidality in autistic people (Cassidy et al., 2018). This suggests that while there may not be quantitative differences in the rates or levels of depression between autistic men, women and NBT people, there are important qualitative differences in behaviour which future work should investigate.

Partially in line with H2, we found that non-autistic men and women scored similarly in terms of their depression levels, with non-autistic NBT people scoring significantly higher – and at a similar level to autistic people. While non-autistic NBT people formed the smallest group in the research, it is notable that this is the group who multiple minority theory is most likely to apply to within the non-autistic sample, as they are a minority within the minority LGBT+ community, and therefore, we would expect them to have elevated levels of mental health issues. NBT people are amongst the most attacked members of the LBGT+ community, and so it may be that they benefit less from the protective effects against depression than those who are cisgender gay (Mclaren et al., 2008) or lesbian (McLaren, 2009).

In line with H3, correlation analyses suggested that for both autistic and non-autistic people, higher levels of autistic traits were associated with higher levels of depression. This may be because those with more challenges in social interaction experience a form of negative reinforcement cycle, for example, they struggle to interact as expected, so get negative responses from peers, leading them to socially withdraw and interact less often, meaning they get less practise, and so struggle to develop better social skills, contributing to depression (Katz et al., 2011).

Eating disorders

As predicted in H1, we found that autistic people endorsed more ED behaviours than non-autistic people of all genders. However, once those with a clinical ED diagnosis were removed from the analysis, no significant difference persisted. This suggests that those on the spectrum may be more likely to fall into the treatment-resistant, severe and enduring group of those with ED, in line with previous research (Westwood and Tchanturia, 2017).

In line with H2, we found that the gendered patterns seen in the general population – i.e. that women are more likely to endorse ED behaviours than men (Andersen and Holman, 1997) – were also true in the autistic sample. Interestingly, NBT people reported ED behaviours at similar rates to women, in line with previous work on EDs amongst a transgender population (Diemer et al., 2015, 2018). This implies that the health inequalities often affecting non-heteronormative populations (Whitehead, 2017; Jones et al., 2019) are also present in EDs, with transgender clients in ED services experiencing a range of unique challenges around clinician assumptions about their bodies and negative experiences with clinicians (Duffy et al., 2016).

Autistic people reported a range of ED diagnoses, not exclusively anorexia nervosa, something which is important for future research to account for as, to date, almost all the literature in this field has focussed on AN to the detriment of our understanding of other diagnoses. There is extensive work documenting the links between autism and AN (Oldershaw et al., 2011; Westwood and Tchanturia, 2017). However, our important addition to the work previously published (Gesi et al., 2017; Westwood and Tchanturia, 2017) shows that the links between autism and AN are not restricted to women. The correlation analyses, in line with H3, suggested a relationship between higher levels of autistic traits and higher levels of ED traits both autistic and non-autistic participants, suggesting that autism screening may be a valuable addition to clinical assessments, as there is evidence that treatment should be adapted for those with high levels of autistic traits, for example, avoiding or adapting the talking therapies which are common approached to ED treatment, as they may be difficult for those with communication differences (Spain et al., 2015).

This study has some limitations. Firstly, the data is entirely self-report. As the data was collected through an online study, we were not able to verify diagnoses with clinicians, but the AQ and EDE-Q scores of the groups provide support for the accuracy of people’s self-report. It is also worth noting that autistic women (and, potentially, non-binary people) face unique challenges to accessing a formal autism diagnosis (Bargiela et al., 2016; Gould and Ashton-Smith, 2011), and so the authors made a conscious decision to respect self-reported autistic identity in this study. While AQ generally has good specificity and validity, there have been critiques made of both its accuracy amongst non-male groups (Murray et al., 2017). However, currently this measure is one of the most accurate and widely used in autism research, and so is justified in its usage in this paper. Secondly, the groups are not matched on demographic variables such as gender. This is to be expected, however, considering work showing that autistic people are more likely to be gender non-conforming than non-autistic people (George and Stokes, 2016, 2017; Dewinter et al., 2017), and therefore, these differences are representative of the population. Thirdly, we did not collect qualitative data on people’s experiences with their mental health, such as length of illness, treatment experiences or their perception of the causes of their difficulties. Future work should ask these questions to gain greater understanding of the differences in autistic people’s lives which result in their greater chance of developing a range of mental health issues. Finally, it is worth noting that this sample has more autistic women and non-binary people than is usual for autism research, as many studies have all- or majority-male samples (Banach et al., 2009). This gender balance is normal for survey studies, however, (Sax et al., 2008), and may be a feature of self-selecting samples as autistic women are more active in the online communities where this study was advertised.

In conclusion, this is the first study to examine the self-reported rates of anxiety, depression and different types of EDs amongst autistic and non-autistic people of all genders. We found that autistic adults are more likely to report having not only anxiety and depression but also every type of ED, going beyond existing work on anorexia nervosa, and that the gendered patterns of ED in the non-autistic population are also seen in the autistic population. These findings have significant implications, suggesting that future work on autism and mental health should consider gender, including non-binary and trans identities, as an important factor in people’s experiences. This consideration of the interaction between gender and autism as shaping experiences should cover everything from healthcare and mental health support, through to interactions with employers and the criminal justice system. Different presentations and behaviours in these situations can have significant impact on whether someone gets the best support and outcomes, and so research should work to provide an evidence base for understanding the best practise for working with autistic people of all genders.

Figures

Demographic characteristics by group (autistic, non-autistic)

Autistic (n = 526) Non-autistic (n = 420)
Age
Range 18.32-71.53 18.29-81.29
M (SD) 33.76 (11.18) 32.08 (10.56)
Gender
Male 67 (12.74%) 56 (13.33%)
Female 314 (59.70%) 345 (82.14%)
Non-binary 145 (27.57%) 19 (4.53%)
Ethnicity
White 389 (73.95%) 326 (77.62%)
Asian 10 (1.90%) 25 (5.95%)
Black 4 (0.76%) 4 (0.95%)
Latinx 5 (0.95%) 2 (0.48%)
Mixed 29 (5.51%) 13 (3.10%)
No answer 89 (16.92%) 50 (11.90%)
Education level
None 22 (4.18%) 2 (0.48%)
GCSE 18 (3.42%) 5 (1.19%)
A-level 83 (15.78%) 35 (8.33%)
Diploma/BTEC 61 (11.60%) 15 (3.57%)
Bachelors degree 204 (38.78%) 181 (43.10%)
Masters degree 111 (21.10%) 143 (34.05%)
PhD 24 (4.56%) 37 (8.81%)
No answer 2 (0.38%) 2 (0.48%)
Employment status
Full-time 134 (25.47%) 215 (51.19%)
Part-time 67 (12.74%) 38 (9.05%)
Student 104 (19.77%) 116 (27.62%)
Self-employed 57 (10.84%) 17 (4.05%)
Unemployed 102 (19.39%) 18 (4.29%)
Retired 11 (2.09%) 4 (0.95%)
Other 48 (9.13%) 10 (2.38%)
No answer 3 (0.57%) 2 (0.48%)
AQ score
Range 3-27 0-27
M (SD) 21.10 (3.76) 9.65 (5.92)

Autistic Non-autistic Test statistic, p-value
Female Male NBT Female Male NBT
HADS anxiety
M (SD)
12.88(4.25) 11.33(5.37) 12.22(4.49) 8.36(4.70) 6.75(3.93) 10.83(4.74) Autism-status: F(1) = 187.81, p < 0.001
Gender: F(2) = 5.37, p = 0.005
Autism-status × gender: F(2) = 2.81, p = 0.060
HADS depression
M (SD)
7.91(3.96) 7.76(4.55) 7.46(4.04) 4.55(3.89) 4.56(3.14) 7.78(4.78) Autism-status: F(1) = 126.78, p < 0.001
Gender: F(2) = 0.07, p = 0.936
Autism-status × Gender: F(2) = 6.01, p = 0.003
EDEQ total score
M (SD)
2.30(1.60) 1.80(1.37) 1.88(1.39) 2.01(1.45) 1.38( 0.99) 1.70(1.13) Autism-status: F(1) = 8.74, p = 0.003
Gender: F(2) = 9.00, p < 0.001
Autism-status × Gender: F(2) = 0.14, p = 0.870
ED diagnosis
N (%)
190 (36.1%)
AN 74 (14.1%)
BN/BED 45 (8.6%)
EDNOS/OSFED 36 (6.8%)
ARFID 11 (2.1%)
Orthorexia 5 (0.9%)
Other ED 15 (2.9%)
83 (20.0%)
AN 52 (12.4%)
BN/BED 26 (6.2%)
EDNOS/OSFED 8 (1.9%)
ARFID 0 (0.0%)
Orthorexia 3 (0.7%)
Other ED 5 (1.2%)
Autism-status: Z = −5.44, p < 0.001
Notes:

NBT = non-binary/transgender; HADS = hospital anxiety and depression scale; EDEQ = eating disorder examination questionnaire; ED = eating disorder; AN = anorexia nervosa; BN = bulimia nervosa; BED = binge eating disorder; EDNOS = eating disorder no otherwise specified; OSFED = other specified feeding or eating disorder; ARFID = avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder; M = mean; SD = standard deviation. The number of specific diagnoses reported by participants does not add up to the total number of people reporting having an eating disorder as these numbers include past and present diagnoses the person has received

References

Andersen, A.E. and Holman, J.E. (1997), “Males with eating disorders: challenges for treatment and research”, Psychopharmacology Bulletin, Vol. 33 No. 3, pp. 391-397.

Baldwin, S. and Costley, D. (2016), “The experiences and needs of female adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder”, Autism, Vol. 20 No. 4, pp. 483-495, doi: 10.1177/1362361315590805.

Banach, R., et al. (2009), “Brief report: relationship between non-verbal IQ and gender in autism”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 39 No. 1, pp. 188-193, doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0612-4.

Bargiela, S., Steward, R. and Mandy, W. (2016), “The experiences of late-diagnosed women with Autism spectrum conditions: an investigation of the female autism phenotype”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 46 No. 10, pp. 3281-3294, doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2872-8.

Bejerot, S., Eriksson, J.M. and Mörtberg, E. (2014), “Social anxiety in adult autism spectrum disorder”, Psychiatry Research, Vol. 220 Nos 1/2, pp. 705-707, doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.08.030.

Botha, M. and Frost, D.M. (2018), “Extending the minority stress model to understand mental health problems experienced by the autistic population”, Society and Mental Health, Vol. 10 No. 1, doi: 10.1177/2156869318804297.

Brody, D.J. Pratt, L.A. and Hughes, J.P. (2013), “Prevalence of depression among adults aged 20 and over: United States, 2013-2016 key findings data from the national health and nutrition examination survey”, available at: www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/databriefs/db303_table.pdf#3 (accessed 12 March 2020).

Budge, S.L., Rossman, H.K. and Howard, K.A.S. (2014), “Coping and psychological distress among genderqueer individuals: the moderating effect of social support”, Journal of LGBT Issues in Counseling, Vol. 8 No. 1, pp. 95-117, doi: 10.1080/15538605.2014.853641.

Camm-Crosbie, L., et al. (2019), “People like me don’t get support”: autistic adults’ experiences of support and treatment for mental health difficulties, self-injury and suicidality”, Autism, Vol. 23 No. 6, doi: 10.1177/1362361318816053.

Cassidy, S., et al. (2018), “Risk markers for suicidality in autistic adults”, Molecular Autism, Vol. 9 No. 1, doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-0226-4.

Cassidy, S. and Rodgers, J. (2017), “Understanding and prevention of suicide in autism”, The Lancet Psychiatry, Vol. 4 No. 6, pp. e11, doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30162-1.

Cermak, S.A., Curtin, C. and Bandini, L.G. (2010), “Food selectivity and sensory sensitivity in children with autism spectrum disorders”, Journal of the American Dietetic Association, Vol. 110 No. 2, pp. 238-246, doi: 10.1016/J.JADA.2009.10.032.

Chistol, L.T., et al. (2018), “Sensory sensitivity and food selectivity in children with autism spectrum disorder”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 48 No. 2, pp. 583-591, doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3340-9.

Crane, L., et al. (2019), “Something needs to change”: mental health experiences of young autistic adults in England”, Autism, Vol. 23 No. 2, doi: 10.1177/1362361318757048.

Crompton, C. (2019), “Efficiency and interaction during information transfer between autistic and neurotypical people”, IMFAR.

Crompton, C.J. Fletcher-Watson, S. and Ropar, D. (2019), “I never realised everybody felt as happy as I do when I am around autistic people”: a thematic analysis of autistic adults’ relationships with autistic and neurotypical friends and family”, OSF Preprints. 10.31219/OSF.IO/46B87.

Dewinter, J., De Graaf, H. and Begeer, S. (2017), “Sexual orientation, gender identity, and romantic relationships in adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 47 No. 9, pp. 2927-2934, doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3199-9.

Diemer, E.W., et al. (2015), “Gender identity, sexual orientation, and Eating-Related pathology in a national sample of college students”, Journal of Adolescent Health, Vol. 57 No. 2, pp. 144-149, doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.03.003.

Diemer, E.W., et al. (2018), “Beyond the binary: differences in eating disorder prevalence by gender identity in a transgender sample”, Transgender Health, Vol. 3 No. 1, pp. 17-23., doi: 10.1089/trgh.2017.0043.

Dovey, T.M., Kumari, V. and Blissett, J. (2019), “Eating behaviour, behavioural problems and sensory profiles of children with avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (ARFID), autistic spectrum disorders or picky eating: same or different?”, European Psychiatry, Vol. 61, pp. 56-62, doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2019.06.008.

Duffy, M.E., Henkel, K.E. and Earnshaw, V.A. (2016), “Transgender clients’ experiences of eating disorder treatment”, Journal of LGBT Issues in Counseling, Vol. 10 No. 3, pp. 136-149, doi: 10.1080/15538605.2016.1177806.

Fairburn, C.G. and Beglin, S.J. (1994), “Assessment of eating disorders: interview or self‐report questionnaire?”, International Journal of Eating Disorders, Vol. 16 No. 4, pp. 363-370.

Fombonne, E. (2003), “The prevalence of autism”, JAMA, Vol. 289 No. 1, p. 87, doi: 10.1001/jama.289.1.87.

George, R. and Stokes, M. (2016), ‘“Gender is not on My agenda!”: gender dysphoria and autism spectrum disorder”, Psychiatric Symptoms and Comorbidities in Autism Spectrum Disorder, Springer International Publishing. Cham, pp. 139-150. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-29695-1_10.

George, R. and Stokes, M.A. (2017), “Gender identity and sexual orientation in autism spectrum disorder”, Autism, Vol. 22 No. 8, doi: 10.1177/1362361317714587.

Gesi, C., et al. (2017), “Autistic traits in patients with anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder: a pilot study”, European Psychiatry, Vol. 41, pp. S100, doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.01.310.

Gillberg, C. (1985), “Autism and anorexia nervosa: related conditions?”, Nordisk Psykiatrisk Tidsskrift, Vol. 39 No. 4, pp. 307-312.

Gillott, A. and Standen, P.J. (2007), “Levels of anxiety and sources of stress in adults with autism”, Journal of Intellectual Disabilities, Vol. 11 No. 4, pp. 359-370, doi: 10.1177/1744629507083585.

Gillott, A., Furniss, F. and Walter, A. (2001), “Anxiety in High-Functioning children with autism”, Autism, Vol. 5 No. 3, pp. 277-286, doi: 10.1177/1362361301005003005.

Godart, N.T., et al. (2007), “Comorbidity studies of eating disorders and mood disorders. Critical review of the literature”, Journal of Affective Disorders, Vol. 97 Nos 1/3, pp. 37-49, doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.06.023.

Gould, J. (2017), “Towards understanding the under-recognition of girls and women on the autism spectrum”, Autism, Vol. 21 No. 6, doi: 10.1177/1362361317706174.

Gould, J. and Ashton-Smith, J. (2011), “Missed diagnosis or misdiagnosis? Girls and women on the autism spectrum”, BILD – The British Institute of Learning Disabilities. available at: www.ingentaconnect.com/content/bild/gap/2011/00000012/00000001/art00005 (accessed 18 October 2018).

Griffiths, S., et al. (2019), “The vulnerability experiences quotient (VEQ): a study of vulnerability, mental health and life satisfaction in autistic adults”, Autism Research, Vol. 12 No. 10, doi: 10.1002/aur.2162.

Haney, J.L. (2016), “Autism, females, and the DSM-5: gender bias in autism diagnosis”, Social Work in Mental Health, Vol. 14 No. 4, pp. 396-407, doi: 10.1080/15332985.2015.1031858.

Heasman, B. and Gillespie, A. (2018), “Neurodivergent intersubjectivity: distinctive features of how autistic people create shared understanding”, Autism, Vol. 23 No. 4, doi: 10.1177/1362361318785172.

Hoekstra, R.A., et al. (2011), “The construction and validation of an abridged version of the Autism-Spectrum quotient (AQ-Short)”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 41 No. 5, pp. 589-596, doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1073-0.

Hull, L., et al. (2019), “Development and validation of the camouflaging autistic traits questionnaire (CAT-Q)”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 49 No. 3, pp. 819-833, doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3792-6.

Jones, B.A., et al. (2019), “Mental health and quality of life in non-binary transgender adults: a case control study”, International Journal of Transgenderism, Vol. 20 Nos 2/3.

Kanfiszer, L., Davies, F. and Collins, S. (2017), “I was just so different”: the experiences of women diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder in adulthood in relation to gender and social relationships”, Autism, Vol. 21 No. 6, pp. 661-669, doi: 10.1177/1362361316687987.

Katz, S.J., et al. (2011), “Childhood social withdrawal, interpersonal impairment, and young adult depression: a mediational model”, Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Vol. 39 No. 8, pp. 1227-1238, doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9537-z.

Kim, J.A., et al. (2000), “The prevalence of anxiety and mood problems among children with autism and asperger syndrome”, Autism, Vol. 4 No. 2, pp. 117-132, doi: 10.1177/1362361300004002002.

Kinnaird, E., et al. (2019), “Eating as an autistic adult: an exploratory qualitative study”, PLoS One, Vol. 14 No. 8, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0221937.

Kuusikko, S., et al. (2008), “Social anxiety in high-functioning children and adolescents with autism and asperger syndrome”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 38 No. 9, pp. 1697-1709, doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0555-9.

Lai, M.-C., et al. (2015), “Sex/gender differences and autism: setting the scene for future research”, Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 54 No. 1, pp. 11-24, doi: 10.1016/J.JAAC.2014.10.003.

Loomes, R., Hull, L. and Mandy, W.P.L. (2017), “What is the male-to-Female ratio in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review and Meta-Analysis”, Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 56 No. 6, pp. 466-474, doi: 10.1016/J.JAAC.2017.03.013.

McLaren, S. (2009), “Sense of belonging to the general and lesbian communities as predictors of depression among lesbians”, Journal of Homosexuality, Vol. 56 No. 1, pp. 1-13, doi: 10.1080/00918360802551365.

Mclaren, S., Jude, B. and Mclachlan, A.J. (2008), “Sense of belonging to the general and gay communities as predictors of depression among Australian gay men”, International Journal of Men's Health, Vol. 7 No. 1, pp. 90-99, doi: 10.3149/jmh.0701.90.

Maddox, B.B. and White, S.W. (2015), “Comorbid social anxiety disorder in adults with autism spectrum disorder”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 45 No. 12, pp. 3949-3960, doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2531-5.

Maechler, M., Rousseeuw, P., Croux, C., Todorov, V., Ruckstuhl, A., Salibian-Barrera, M., Verbeke, T., Koller, M., Conceicao, E.L.T. and di Palma, M.A. (2019), “Package robustbase”, available at: http://robustbase.r-forge.r-project.org/ (accessed 12 March 2020).

Magiati, I., et al. (2016), “Anxiety symptoms in young people with autism spectrum disorder attending special schools: associations with gender, adaptive functioning and autism symptomatology”, Autism, Vol. 20 No. 3, pp. 306-320, doi: 10.1177/1362361315577519.

May, T., Cornish, K. and Rinehart, N. (2014), “Does gender matter? A one year follow-up of autistic, attention and anxiety symptoms in high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 44 No. 5, pp. 1077-1086, doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1964-y.

Meyer, I.H. (1995), “Minority stress and mental health in gay men”, Journal of Health and Social Behavior, Vol. 36 No. 1, p. 38, doi: 10.2307/2137286.

Meyer, I.H. (2015), “Resilience in the study of minority stress and health of sexual and gender minorities”, Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, Vol. 2 No. 3, pp. 209-213, doi: 10.1037/sgd0000132.

Micali, N., et al. (2013), “The incidence of eating disorders in the UK in 2000–2009: findings from the general practice research database”, BMJ Open, Vol. 3 No. 5, pp. e002646, doi: 10.1136/BMJOPEN-2013-002646.

Miller, A.F. (2013), “The Non-Religious patriarchy: why losing religion HAS NOT meant losing white male dominance”, CrossCurrents, Vol. 63 No. 2, pp. 211-226., doi: 10.1111/cros.12025.

Murray, A.L., et al. (2017), “Investigating sex bias in the AQ-10: a replication study”, Assessment, Vol. 26 No. 8, doi: 10.1177/1073191117733548.

Oldershaw, A., et al. (2011), “Is anorexia nervosa a version of autism spectrum disorders?”, European Eating Disorders Review, Vol. 19 No. 6, pp. 462-474.

Raevuori, A., Keski-Rahkonen, A. and Hoek, H.W. (2014), “A review of eating disorders in males”, Current Opinion in Psychiatry, Vol. 27 No. 6, pp. 426-430, doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000113.

Remes, O., et al. (2016), “A systematic review of reviews on the prevalence of anxiety disorders in adult populations”, Brain and Behavior, Vol. 6 No. 7, pp. e00497, doi: 10.1002/brb3.497.

Roberts, K.E., Schwartz, D. and Hart, T.A. (2011), “‘Social anxiety among lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender adolescents and young adults’”, Social Anxiety in Adolescents and Young Adults: Translating Developmental Science into Practice, American Psychological Association, London pp. 161-181. doi: 10.1037/12315-009.

Rowley, E., et al. (2012), “The experience of friendship, victimization and bullying in children with an autism spectrum disorder: associations with child characteristics and school placement”, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Vol. 6 No. 3, pp. 1126-1134, doi: 10.1016/J.RASD.2012.03.004.

Sax, L.J., et al. (2008), “Using web surveys to reach community college students: an analysis of response rates and response bias”, Community College Journal of Research and Practice, Vol. 32 No. 9, pp. 712-729, doi: 10.1080/10668920802000423.

Schreck, K.A., Williams, K. and Smith, A.F. (2004), “A comparison of eating behaviors between children with and without autism”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 34 No. 4, pp. 433-438, doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000037419.78531.86.

Smink, F.R.E., van Hoeken, D. and Hoek, H.W. (2012), “Epidemiology of eating disorders: incidence, prevalence and mortality rates”, Current Psychiatry Reports, Vol. 14 No. 4, pp. 406-414, doi: 10.1007/s11920-012-0282-y.

Spain, D., et al. (2015), “Cognitive behaviour therapy for adults with autism spectrum disorders and psychiatric co-morbidity: a review”, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Vol. 9, pp. 151-162, doi: 10.1016/J.RASD.2014.10.019.

Swinbourne, J.M. and Touyz, S.W. (2007), “The co-morbidity of eating disorders and anxiety disorders: a review”, European Eating Disorders Review, Vol. 15 No. 4, pp. 253-274, doi: 10.1002/erv.784.

Uljarević, M., et al. (2018), “The hospital anxiety and depression scale: factor structure and psychometric properties in older adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum disorder”, Autism Research, Vol. 11 No. 2, pp. 258-269, doi: 10.1002/aur.1872.

van Steensel, F.J.A., Bögels, S.M. and Perrin, S. (2011), “Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders: a Meta-Analysis”, Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, Vol. 14 No. 3, pp. 302-317, doi: 10.1007/s10567-011-0097-0.

Venables, V.N. and Ripley, B.D. (2002), Modern Applied Statistics with S. Chambers SJ, Eddy W, Hardle W, Sheater S, Tierney L, Springer Science+ Business Media. New York, NY.

Westwood, H. and Tchanturia, K. (2017), “Autism spectrum disorder in anorexia nervosa: an updated literature review”, Current Psychiatry Reports, Vol. 19 No. 7, p. 41, doi: 10.1007/s11920-017-0791-9.

Westwood, H., Mandy, W. and Tchanturia, K. (2017), “Clinical evaluation of autistic symptoms in women with anorexia nervosa”, Molecular Autism, Vol. 8 No. 1, p. 12, doi: 10.1186/s13229-017-0128-x.

White, S.W., et al. (2009), “Anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders”, Clinical Psychology Review, Vol. 29 No. 3, pp. 216-229, doi: 10.1016/J.CPR.2009.01.003.

Whitehead, B. (2017), “Inequalities in access to health care for transgender patients”, Links to Health and Social Care, Vol. 2 No. 1, pp. 63-76.

Worley, J.A. and Matson, J.L. (2011), “Psychiatric symptoms in children diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder: an examination of gender differences”, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Vol. 5 No. 3, pp. 1086-1091, doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.12.002.

Zigmond, A.S. and Snaith, R.P. (1983), “The hospital anxiety and depression scale”, Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, Vol. 67 No. 6, pp. 361-370.

Corresponding author

Felicity Sedgewick can be contacted at: felicity.sedgewick@bristol.ac.uk

About the authors

Felicity Sedgewick is based at the Department of Education, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK and Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK.

Jenni Leppanen is based at the Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK.

Kate Tchanturia is based at the Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry Psychology and Neuroscience, South London and Maudsley Mental Health NHS Trust, London, UK, and Ilia State University, Tbilisi, Georgia.

Related articles