Alexithymia, social inhibition, affectivity, and knowledge hiding

Roman Kmieciak (Centre for New Technologies, Silesian University of Technology, Gliwice, Poland)

Journal of Knowledge Management

ISSN: 1367-3270

Article publication date: 27 April 2022

Issue publication date: 19 December 2022

1645

Abstract

Purpose

This paper aims to examine the impact of adverse personality traits (alexithymia, social inhibition, negative affectivity) and supervisor knowledge hiding on individual knowledge hiding. This study also explores the moderating role of positive affectivity.

Design/methodology/approach

Partial least squares path modeling and data collected from 518 Polish employees with higher education and extensive professional experience recruited via an Ariadna survey panel were used to test the research hypotheses.

Findings

Two dimensions of alexithymia were considered: difficulty identifying feelings (DIF) and difficulty describing feelings (DDF). DIF has a direct impact on individual hiding, whereas DDF has an indirect impact, via social inhibition. Negative affectivity is a predictor of social inhibition, which enhances knowledge hiding. Positive affectivity slightly weakens the positive and strong effect of supervisor knowledge hiding on subordinate knowledge hiding.

Practical implications

Because alexithymia, social inhibition and negative affectivity may predispose employees to knowledge hiding, managers should identify these personality traits among job applicants and hired employees to make appropriate employment decisions. Moreover, managers should be aware that hiding knowledge by a supervisor may be imitated by subordinates.

Originality/value

Based on conservation of resources theory, this study investigates previously unexplored relationships among alexithymia, social inhibition, affectivity and knowledge hiding.

Keywords

Citation

Kmieciak, R. (2022), "Alexithymia, social inhibition, affectivity, and knowledge hiding", Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 26 No. 11, pp. 461-485. https://doi.org/10.1108/JKM-10-2021-0782

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2022, Roman Kmieciak.

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence maybe seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


1. Introduction

Knowledge hiding (KH) is generally perceived as a counterproductive knowledge behavior (Chen et al., 2022; Kmieciak, 2021; Serenko and Bontis, 2016) and defined as “an intentional attempt by an individual to withhold or conceal knowledge that has been requested by another person” (Connelly et al., 2012, p. 65). With the growing interest in KH in organizations in recent years, various antecedents to KH have been investigated, both at the individual and organizational levels (for a review, see Siachou et al., 2021). There has been relatively little research into the impact of personality traits on counterproductive knowledge behavior in the workplace (Geofroy and Evans, 2017). Nevertheless, it was found that knowledge hiding is related to competitive personality traits (Hernaus et al., 2019) and the dark triad (Machiavellianism, narcissism, psychopathy) (Karim, 2020; Pan et al., 2018). Recently, Serenko and Choo (2020) investigated the effect of the dark triad personality traits on knowledge sabotage, suggesting that other traits, including alexithymia and negative affectivity, may be related to counterproductive knowledge behavior and should be explored in future research. The present study responds to this call.

Alexithymia is viewed as a personality trait that refers to difficulty identifying and describing one’s own feelings (Taylor et al., 1985). This concept was originally used to characterize psychosomatic patients. However, it was recently applied to describe psychological deficits in broader populations (Moriguchi et al., 2007). It is estimated that the prevalence of alexithymia in the general population is 10% (Franz et al., 2008; Mattila et al., 2006) and in the working-age population is 9%–17% for men and 5%–10% for women (Mattila et al., 2007). Previous research found a negative impact of alexithymia on individual’s empathy (Aslan et al., 2021), perspective taking (Banzhaf et al., 2018), altruism (FeldmanHall et al., 2013), interpersonal relationships (Besharat, 2010) and communication abilities (Meganck et al., 2009). These results suggest that alexithymia may also have an impact on knowledge hiding, although there is a lack of empirical research on this issue.

Negative affectivity (NA) is an emotion-based trait that refers to experienced negative moods, including distress, disappointment, hostility, nervousness and anger (Watson et al., 1988). Individuals with high negative affectivity have low self-confidence (Watson and Clark, 1984) and perceive environments as hostile (Thoresen et al., 2003), which may lead to counterproductive work behaviors (Samnani et al., 2014), withdrawal behaviors (Necowitz and Roznowski, 1994) and knowledge hiding (Ma and Zhang, 2021). The link between negative affectivity and knowledge hiding might be indirect. As Ma and Zhang (2021) claimed, high negative affectivity “may be accompanied by the deterioration of a colleague relationship, which leads to the rise of knowledge hiding behavior” (p. 7). Moreover, in psychological research, negative affectivity is often combined with social inhibition, which is characteristic of a distressed or Type D personality (Denollet, 2005). Therefore, social inhibition may play a significant mediating role in the relationship between NA and KH.

In contrast to individuals with high NA, those with high positive affectivity (PA) are optimistic, energetic, and enthusiastic (Watson et al., 1988). As previous research has indicated, the positive attitude may deter employees from their negative response to different unfavorable organizational situations, including adverse organizational politics (Abbas et al., 2014) and contract breaches (Jahanzeb et al., 2020). However, what has not been examined is how individuals with high positive affectivity react when a supervisor hides knowledge from them and the extent to which this is reflected in their knowledge hiding behavior.

The literature review indicates that the effect of alexithymia and affectivity on knowledge hiding is underexplored. To fulfil this research gap, this study proposes a model of relationships between these phenomena. Because alexithymia is a multidimensional construct (Bagby et al., 1994), the study investigates the separate influence of two dimensions of alexithymia; that is, difficulty identifying and describing feelings. Moreover, the model also includes social inhibition, which is expected to mediate relationship between negative affectivity and knowledge hiding. Additionally, it is proposed that positive affectivity moderate the relationships between supervisor knowledge hiding and subordinate’s knowledge hiding from co-workers. Those two latter effects have not also been investigated previously. To provide theoretical arguments about the relationships between the above-mentioned constructs, this study uses conservation of resources (COR) theory (Hobfoll, 1989; Hobfoll et al., 2018). In addition, the effect of supervisor knowledge hiding on subordinates’ knowledge hiding is explained with a theory of behavioral contagion (Wheeler, 1966). The developed model was tested using data collected from 518 employees with high knowledge and professional experience. The results of this study contribute to theory of knowledge management by linking the psychological concepts of alexithymia, social inhibition and affectivity with KH in a novel way. Moreover, the results provide suggestions to managers interested in limiting KH at workplaces.

2. Theoretical background

2.1 Knowledge management and hiding

It is widely recognized that knowledge management contributes to better firm performance and achieving competitive advantage (Cillo et al., 2019, 2021; Rossi et al., 2020; Santoro et al., 2021). Consequently, with the aim of improving the effectiveness of KM, empirical research has sought to explain what influences the willingness of employees to share knowledge. These studies have analyzed various situational, interpersonal and individual factors that favored or inhibited knowledge sharing (Michna and Kmieciak, 2020). However, as Pan et al. (2018) noted, “despite all these efforts, knowledge hiding among employees is still pervasive” (p. 36). Moreover, Caputo et al. (2021) claimed that knowledge hiding “seems to still be a sort of black box in which human resources are trapped” (p. 20). These conclusions result from the fact that knowledge hiding is not the opposite of knowledge sharing. Conceptually, knowledge hiding and knowledge sharing are different constructs and require separate research (Anand et al., 2021).

KH, defined as deliberately concealing requested knowledge (Connelly et al., 2012), is not necessarily intended to harm a person or organization, but is instead a response to a specific situation (Connelly and Zweig, 2015; Koay and Lim, 2021; Xiong et al., 2021). KH is driven by various factors (Anand et al., 2021; Caputo et al., 2021; Koay et al., 2020; Yao et al., 2020) and can be examined from the dispositional, ownership and leadership perspective (Agarwal et al., 2021).

Employee behavior often results from the principle of reciprocity. If an employee encounters knowledge hiding on the part of his colleagues, then he or she will also be more inclined to retain knowledge (Geofroy and Evans, 2017). Retaining knowledge may be caused by time constraints – in conditions of time pressure, employees are willing to focus on tasks that bring them more benefits than sharing knowledge (Michailova and Husted, 2003). Some employees may believe that knowledge becomes less valuable if it is shared with others and that KH is beneficial to their career development, maintaining position, status or power within the company (Koay et al., 2020; Riege, 2005). KH may also result from the employee’s fear that superiors will sabotage the professional promotion of a subordinate if they consider the subordinate to be more knowledgeable than them (Michailova and Husted, 2003).

The dispositional perspective assumes that some personality traits may predispose to knowledge hiding behavior (Agarwal et al., 2021). In line with this perspective, some studies have started to investigate knowledge hiding in relation to various personality traits (Arshad and Ismail, 2018; Hernaus et al., 2019; Karim, 2020; Pan et al., 2018). For example, Pan et al. (2018) noted that different dimensions of the dark triad are significantly and positively related to different knowledge hiding strategies (evasive hiding, playing dumb and rationalized hiding). Belschak et al. (2018) claimed that the impact of Machiavellianism on knowledge hiding is weakened under highly ethical leaders. Arshad and Ismail (2018) found that the effect of workplace incivility on knowledge hiding is moderated by neuroticism.

2.2 Conservation of resources theory perspective

According to conservation of resources (COR) theory, “people strive to retain, protect, and build resources and that what is threatening to them is the potential or actual loss of these valued resources” (Hobfoll, 1989, p. 516). Resources are viewed very broadly as valued entities including objects (such as tools for work), conditions (such as employment), personal characteristics (personal traits, key skills, etc.) and energy resources (such as knowledge) (Hobfoll, 2001; Hobfoll et al., 2018). Hence, resources also include psychological resources (hope, sense of optimism, positive feeling about oneself, etc.) and social resources (such as support from co-workers and the ability to communicate well) (Hobfoll, 2001).

The present paper has argued that alexithymics and socially inhibited individuals have psychological and social resources deficiencies, which influence their knowledge investment choices. In particular, they have a limited ability to identify and express their feelings, communicate and/or socialize. That sense of resource deficiencies may lead them to conserve other resources, such as knowledge. So, although they may have appropriate knowledge, skills and time, they may hide knowledge, because knowledge sharing is viewed as resource expenditure. However, the COR theory also assumes that “people must invest resources in order to protect against resource loss, recover from losses, and gain resources” (Hobfoll et al., 2018, p. 106). So, knowledge sharing and building interpersonal relations can be viewed as investments that may help people gain resources. For example, it was found that knowledge sharing has a positive impact on knowledge reciprocation (Radaelli et al., 2014), the quality of social relationships with co-workers, and subjective well-being (Jiang and Hu, 2016). Gaining social resources might be particularly appealing to alexithymics and socially inhibited individuals. However, they may be not sufficiently interested in such potential gain because of their strong sense of resources deficiencies and fear of further resource loss. According to COR theory, individuals with low levels of resources are more sensitive to resource loss and have lower ability to gain resources than those with higher levels of resources (Hobfoll et al., 2018). This is because the low-resource individuals are concerned about the risks of investing in new resources and losing the rest of their own resources. Therefore, this study proposes that alexithymics and socially inhibited individuals have a tendency to KH because they are more interested in resource protection than in resource gain.

2.3 Alexithymia

The term “alexithymic” was coined by Sifneos (1973) to describe individuals who were unable to find appropriate words for their moods and emotions. Later, Sifneos (1991) described alexithymia as deficits that involved “an inability to identify and use language to describe feelings, inability to differentiate between emotions with their bodily sensations and feelings, paucity of dreams and fantasy life, and a tendency to describe endless details surrounding a particular emotion-arousing episode which was referred to as an operational way of thinking or as ‘pensee operatoire’” (p. 118). Scholars have since distinguished between primary and secondary alexithymia. Primary alexithymia is considered as a stable personality trait that is formed in childhood and early adulthood, while secondary alexithymia results from somatic disease, psychological stress or other sociocultural and psychodynamic factors (Goerlich, 2018; Tolmunen et al., 2011). Alexithymia appears to be a stable trait across the lifespan in general healthy populations (Morie et al., 2016; Palma-Álvarez et al., 2021).

The alexithymia construct includes three related factors: difficulty identifying feelings (DIF), difficulty describing feelings (DDF) and externally orientated thinking style (EOT) (Bagby et al., 1994). DIF reflects the extent to which people have difficulty identifying their own inner emotional states and distinguishing between feelings and bodily sensations. DDF reflects one’s ability to find the right words to name their own feelings and to verbalize those feelings to other people. As Goerlich (2018) noted, DIF and DDF are usually highly correlated because identifying a feeling requires labeling the feeling. Finally, the third factor of alexithymia, EOT, reflects the degree to which people focus on the specific details of external actions rather than the analysis of their own and others’ feelings, emotions and other aspects of internal experiences. DIF, DDF and EOT are perceived as three cognitive alexithymia facets (Goerlich-Dobre et al., 2014).

2.4 Alexithymia and knowledge hiding

Although there is no empirical research on a relationship between alexithymia and knowledge hiding, there are premises suggesting the existence of such a relationship. Alexithymia is accompanied by a specific style of interpersonal relations. This is because alexithymics find it difficult not only to identify and name their own emotions but also, by extension, to distinguish and appreciate other people’s emotions (Cook et al., 2013; Scheerer et al., 2021). Consequently, alexithymia is negatively related to social competence (Scheerer et al., 2021), including empathy (Aslan et al., 2021; Moriguchi et al., 2007) and altruism (FeldmanHall et al., 2013).

First, alexithymics have a deficit in recognizing their own emotional states, which leads to an impairment in empathy (Moriguchi et al., 2007). As Moriguchi et al. (2007) argued, “Self-awareness is a fundamental aspect of empathy because the individual’s recognition of their own feelings is the basis for identification with the feelings of others” (p. 2223). Previous empirical research has confirmed that alexithymia is inversely related to empathy (Aslan et al., 2021; Guttman and Laporte, 2002). Empathy, including empathic concern and perspective taking, determines interpersonal relationships and good empathy skills make it possible to identify and understand other people’s needs and preferences (Aslan et al., 2021). Empathic people have an inner desire to reduce others’ distress and help others improve their situation (Decety et al., 2016). Hence, they are more eager and willing to respond to other people’s knowledge needs and share knowledge (van den Hooff et al., 2012). Moreover, as Škerlavaj et al. (2018) found, employees who engage in perspective taking are expected to avoid hiding knowledge from co-workers. On the other hand, alexithymic employees, who have difficulty identifying and understanding other people’s emotions, expectations, and situations, can remain indifferent to requests for knowledge and hide knowledge from co-workers.

Second, alexithymics behave less altruistically because of emotional blunting. As FeldmanHall et al. (2013) explained, alexithymics have impaired cognitive mechanisms to identify and register distress of others, which means they are less sensitive and responsive to others’ distress. A diminished distress response results in less prosocial choices and more self-serving behavior. Thus, the inability to read the emotions and distress of co-workers suffering from a lack of adequate knowledge may contribute to selfish behaviors and, consequently, hiding knowledge. As Riege (2005) noted, such selfish behaviors as protection of one’s corporate position, power or status might be reasons for knowledge hoarding. In turn, altruism was found to be positively related to knowledge sharing (Obrenovic et al., 2020). Taking into account the above argumentation, it is assumed that one of the three dimensions of alexithymia – difficulty identifying feelings – might be particularly related to knowledge hiding. Hence, the following hypothesis was stated:

H1.

Difficulty identifying feelings has a positive effect on individual knowledge hiding.

2.5 Social inhibition

Social inhibition reflects “the tendency to inhibit the expression of emotions/behaviors in social interactions to avoid disapproval by others” (Denollet, 2005, p. 89). More recently, it has been described as a stable personality trait among adults that manifests through three facets: behavioral inhibition, social-evaluative concerns and social withdrawal (Denollet and Duijndam, 2019). These three facets reflect, respectively, behavioral, cognitive and affective characteristics of social inhibition. Behavioral inhibition is related with difficulties making contact and conversation with others, less talkability, hesitation in speech, and inhibition in presenting own ideas. Social-evaluative concerns refers to interpersonal sensitivity, including concerns about negative social evaluation and fear of criticism from others. Finally, social withdrawal is a self-enhancing strategy to avoid social interactions when negative reactions from others and social stress are expected. Generally, socially inhibited individuals feel uncomfortable around other people and lead less active social lives (Gest, 1997; de Moor et al., 2018). When social interaction is expected, they may use various safety behaviors, such as looking away or remaining in the background, to reduce the level of anxiety (Duijndam et al., 2021).

2.6 Alexithymia and social inhibition

One of the dimensions of alexithymia is difficulty describing feelings (DDF), which means that alexithymics have difficulty with the cognitive processing of emotions and verbalizing their own feelings and emotions. Difficulty with the cognitive processing of own emotions hinders the development of other cognitive abilities, such as identifying and describing emotion in others. This inability to describe oneself and the environment can negatively affect communication and interaction with other people (Scheerer et al., 2021). Alexithymics may feel that they are not good conversation partners because they cannot express their feelings and therefore do not meet the expectations of the interlocutor. Moreover, alexithymics have problems recognizing emotion in others (Cook et al., 2013), which may cause their response to another person’s emotions to be inappropriate or not as expected. Consequently, alexithymics may feel psychological discomfort in relationships with others. As Scheerer et al. (2021) noted, “emotional awareness and understanding are essential aspects of social engagement and understanding” (p. 1253). Meganck et al. (2009) found that alexithymics use a less complex vocabulary to describe their relationships with others. Summarizing previous research, Vanheule et al. (2007) reported that alexithymics tend to avoid close social relationships and treat others in a cold, distant and unempathetic manner. Thus, this imperfection in communication skills, describing and interpreting emotions, observed among alexithymics, can cause alexithymics to experience frustration and disappointment among their social environment. As a consequence, alexithymics may lead a limited social life, including meeting new people and creating close relationships with them. Therefore, the following hypothesis is proposed:

H2.

Difficulty describing feelings has a positive effect on social inhibition.

2.7 Positive and negative affectivity

Affectivity can be defined as “an emotion-based trait dimension that creates a cognitive bias through which individuals approach and understand experiences and may affect how they experience and evaluate jobs” (Naquin and Holton, 2002, p. 359). The dominant view, popularized by Watson et al. (1988), is that affectivity encompasses two basic distinctive dimensions: positive affectivity (PA) and negative affectivity (NA). PA refers to experiencing positive emotional states and pleasant engagement with the environment. Individuals with high PA are energetic, enthusiastic, active and alert, whereas those with low PA are sad and lethargic. In contrast, NA is accompanied by dysphoria and a conviction of impending trouble (Denollet, 2005). High-NA individuals concentrate more on negative aspects of life, their own mistakes, disappointments and weaknesses (Naquin and Holton, 2002). They experience different aversive mood states such as shame, hostility, nervousness, fear, and anger, whereas those with low NA are calm and untroubled (Watson et al., 1988).

2.8 Negative affectivity and social inhibition

Negative affectivity may have a significant effect on social interactions. First, individuals with high negative affectivity have a negative self-concept and tend to dwell on adverse facets of themselves. Focusing on their own mistakes, shortcoming and disappointments may be linked to low self-esteem and self-confidence (Watson and Clark, 1984), which are barriers to building interpersonal relationships (Harris and Orth, 2020). People with low self-esteem and a lack of self-confidence fear social rejection (Don et al., 2019; Zhou et al., 2020) and tend to avoid communication situations (Oktary et al., 2019; Pearson et al., 2011), hence they may be socially inhibited. Second, high-NA individuals tend to focus on the negative aspects of other people and the world in general. They perceive their environment as more hostile and threatening (Thoresen et al., 2003). In situations requiring the interpretation of other people’s behavior, they are more likely to attribute malicious motives to others (Penney and Spector, 2005). Subordinates who are high on NA perceive their supervisor’s punishment actions as harsher, which leads to less trusting in supervisor and less organizational commitment (Ball et al., 1993). Moreover, in response to perceived organizational unfairness, high-NA individuals are more likely to engage in organizational retaliatory behavior (Skarlicki et al., 1999). High-NA individuals are predisposed to perceive work and co-workers unfavorably and dwell on instances of negative interpersonal treatment (Bowling et al., 2008). A meta-analysis conducted by Thoresen et al. (2003) revealed that NA was positively related to emotional exhaustion, depersonalization (that is, treating others as objects, in a callous and cynical manner) and turnover intention, but negatively related to job satisfaction, organizational commitment and personal accomplishment.

Even in a pleasant workplace and in the absence of objective stressors, high-NA individuals are likely to feel more distress, frustration and dissatisfaction than individuals with low negative affectivity (Necowitz and Roznowski, 1994). Therefore, the following hypothesis is proposed:

H3.

Negative affectivity has a positive effect on social inhibition.

2.9 Social inhibition and knowledge hiding

Personality traits are significantly related to willingness to share or hide knowledge (Agyemang et al., 2016; Pan et al., 2018). One of these traits might be social inhibition. The impact of social inhibition on knowledge hiding can be explained through two intertwined mechanisms. First, socially inhibited individuals avoid socializing. Therefore, they have less opportunity to learn about behaviors and feelings of others, to develop empathy, mutual trust and reciprocal behavior. As research has shown, high-inhibited individuals have less empathy and less ability to solve social problem (Bengtsgard and Bohlin, 2001) and an increase in social withdrawal leads to a decrease in social trust (Aboutorab and Raziyeh, 2012). Moreover, scholars have highlighted the importance of empathy and trust for reducing knowledge hiding (Geofroy and Evans, 2017). For example, it is advised to provide opportunities for socializing beyond work to form trust between employees and overcome knowledge hiding (Anand and Hassan, 2019). Hence, avoiding socializing and its results in terms of lack of trust and empathy might be predictors of knowledge hiding. Second, socially inhibited individuals are shy and feel uncomfortable around other people and having difficulty initiating and conducting conversations (Duijndam et al., 2021). Thus, they may be reluctant to speak up and present their own initiatives, ideas and solutions. Consequently, although they have appropriate and desired knowledge, they can deliberately avoid social interactions that would require sharing their knowledge with others. In line with this idea, previous studies have revealed that shyness and lack of confidence are important barriers to face-to-face knowledge sharing (Majid et al., 2015; Yao et al., 2007). Therefore, the following hypothesis was put forward:

H4.

Social inhibition has a positive effect on knowledge hiding.

2.10 Supervisor knowledge hiding and knowledge hiding

Under social exchange theory (Cropanzano and Mitchell, 2005) and the norm of reciprocity (Gouldner, 1960), we tend to reciprocate other people’s behavior toward us. Therefore, if we know or suspect that other people are hiding knowledge from us, then we are more likely to hide our knowledge from those people. Empirical research confirms this reciprocity (Černe et al., 2014; Serenko and Bontis, 2016). However, an interesting question is whether the superior’s hiding knowledge from us influences our decisions about hiding knowledge from co-workers who are not our superiors. A conversation of resource theory (Hobfoll, 1989) and a theory of behavioral contagion (Wheeler, 1966) may be helpful to explain this influence.

First, according to COR theory, people fear they will lose their resources (Hobfoll, 1989). One of the threats are stressful work conditions. When confronted with such conditions, employees strive to minimize loss of resources. In line with this logic, in response to stress and other adverse organizational situations, employees can become involved in deviant work behaviors to maintain a sense of self-worth (Jahanzeb et al., 2020). For example, if employees have committed a lot of energy to work, and the organization does not honor its obligations to them, then employees may engage in knowledge-hiding behavior to avoid further resource losses, maintain self-esteem and control over own knowledge (Jahanzeb et al., 2020). Supervisor knowledge hiding may be perceived as a stressful work condition and as a threat to resources possessed by subordinates, such as knowledge, mastery and status within the organization. A supervisor who hides knowledge from his or her subordinates deprives them of the chance to update their knowledge. Consequently, subordinates may respond by protecting and hiding their own knowledge resources from co-workers to maintain or improve their status within the organization.

Second, behavioral contagion refers to the tendency or process in which certain behaviors exhibited by an actor are copied by observers who are socially related to the original actor (Heng et al., 2019; Liao et al., 2020). However, in contrast to other types of social influence, such as social pressures or conformity, in behavioral contagion the actor does not intend to evoke such copying process (Wheeler, 1966). As antisocial behaviors might be more contagious than pro-social ones (Dimant, 2019), prior research has typically used a theory of behavioral contagion to explain spreading undesirable behaviors (Heng et al., 2019). It has been confirmed that employees may have a tendency to imitate undesirable or even deviant behaviors at the workplace (Gino et al., 2009; Robinson et al., 2014). For example, individuals imitate coworkers’ routine safety violations (Liang et al., 2018) and withdrawal behavior (Eder and Eisenberger, 2008). Recently, Serenko and Choo (2020) used a theory of behavioral contagion to justify the positive effect of a co-worker’s knowledge sabotage on an individual’s knowledge sabotage.

According to the theory of behavioral contagion, individuals may experience an internal conflict between certain behavior and internal restraints (Wheeler, 1966). In such a situation, they are not sure if they can perform this behavior. However, if they observe this behavior among other people, then the internal mechanisms of resistance to such behavior weaken. Cognitive mechanisms such as moral justification are activated, which allows people to engage in unethical behavior without feeling guilty (Ayub et al., 2021; Bandura, 1999). Consequently, individuals may imitate the behavior of others. Behavioral contagion occurs when the initiator of the behavior and the follower are in a similar situation (Ogunlade, 1979). This condition is met at the workplace. Moreover, superiors are usually well suited as role models and closely observed by subordinates (Offergelt et al., 2019). Therefore, it can be assumed that a subordinate from whom the superior hides knowledge may reduce internal restraints against knowledge hiding. Consequently, the subordinate is likely to mimic a superior’s behavior and hide knowledge from co-workers. This is in line with the findings of Liu et al. (2020), who claimed that “if the status of an individual with knowledge concealment is higher, the behavior is more likely to be imitated by team members” (p. 8). Hence, the following hypothesis is proposed:

H5.

Supervisor knowledge hiding has a positive effect on subordinate’s knowledge hiding from co-workers.

2.11 The moderating role of positive affectivity

Conservation of resources theory assumes that the negative reactions of employees to unfavorable, stressful working conditions differ depending on their access to valuable personal resources (Hobfoll, 2001; Jahanzeb et al., 2020). In the present study, it is proposed that positive affectivity is a personal resource that should moderate the relationship between supervisor knowledge hiding and subordinate’s knowledge hiding from co-workers. Individuals with high positive affectivity are full of optimism, energy and enthusiasm (Watson et al., 1988). Their positive mood may serve as a buffer against stress and anxiety caused by various adverse organizational situations (Abbas et al., 2014; Jahanzeb et al., 2020), including supervisor knowledge hiding. Therefore, they may feel less need to react by hiding knowledge from co-workers as a way of preserving their resources and maintaining their influence and status within the organization. Individuals with high positive affectivity may be more willing to believe that a supervisor has important reasons for hiding knowledge and will hence be more likely to forgive or accept supervisor knowledge hiding. Moreover, they are more likely to believe that they can manage without this hidden knowledge or gain it in some other way. Therefore, positive affectivity is a personality trait that allows employees to adapt and deal more effectively with the distress related to supervisor knowledge hiding, so their reaction in the form of hiding knowledge from co-workers to protect their resources may become less important. Previous research has confirmed that positive affectivity can moderate the relationship between psychological contract breaches and knowledge hiding (Jahanzeb et al., 2020). Similarly, it was found that optimism weakens the association between competitive psychological climate and knowledge hiding (Han et al., 2020). These findings suggest the following hypothesis:

H6.

Positive affectivity moderates the relationship between supervisor knowledge hiding and subordinate’s knowledge hiding from co-workers, in that the positive relationship will be weaker at higher levels of positive affectivity.

Figure 1 presents the research model.

3. Methodology

3.1 Sample and procedures

The data were collected using a questionnaire and in cooperation with the Polish online research platform Ariadna (panelariadna.pl). Ariadna National Research Panel is a professional and certificated online survey company that has more than 150,000 active members. The company sent the questionnaire to potential respondents among its registered users. After completing the questionnaire, each respondent received points that can be exchanged for prizes. A similar data collection method with a paid sample service has been in previous studies on undesirable knowledge behaviors (Ma et al., 2020; Wu, 2020). This data collection method was selected for several reasons (Serenko and Choo, 2020). First, it makes it possible to obtain a large sample size quickly. Second, it ensures anonymity of respondents, which is important in studies on undesirable knowledge behaviors, such as knowledge hiding. Third, respondents come from different geographical locations and have diversified backgrounds, which improves the generalizability of research findings. Finally, this method makes it possible to implement many screening criteria. In this study, only the following respondents were qualified for the study: those with higher education, full-time employees, working in the private sector in enterprises employing at least 10 employees and with at least two years of work experience in the enterprise in which they currently work. These criteria were intended to ensure that the respondents were employees with high knowledge and experience. Data collection lasted for one week in May 2021. At that time, 4,149 people agreed to participate in the study, but 3,529 did not meet the screening criteria and 102 stopped filling out the questionnaire. Finally, 518 valid questionnaires were obtained.

Among the 518 participants, 63.9% were female. Respondents’ average age was 38 years, ranging from 22 to 75. The majority of participants (41.1%) were between the ages of 25 and 34. Respondents’ average job tenure in their current organization was seven years, ranging from 2 to 41. All respondents were highly educated: 25.5% had a bachelor’s degree, 71.0% had a master’s degree and 3.5% had a PhD.

The study was based on self-reported responses and a single questionnaire. Therefore, it is necessary to check whether the common method bias (CMB) has a serious impact on the study results. For this purpose, a Harman single-factor test was applied (Podsakoff et al., 2003). Single factor was extracting 22.89% of the total variance. As this figure is less than 50%, it can be concluded that there is no threat of a significant CMB in this study.

3.2 Measures

All of the scales used in this study were previously established and validated. A full list of scale items is presented in the Appendix.

Alexithymia. Polish language version of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale TAS-20 (www.researchgate.net/publication/342330387) was used to measure alexithymia. Both the original English version (Bagby et al., 1994) and the Polish translation of the TAS-20 (Ścigała et al., 2020) were found to be reliable and valid measures of alexithymia. TAS-20 is a self-report instrument and consists of three subscales:

  1. difficulty identifying feelings (DIF, seven items);

  2. difficulty describing feelings (DDF, five items); and

  3. externally oriented thinking (EOT, eight items).

In this study the first two subscales of TAS-20 were analyzed; that is, DIF and DDF. Each item was scored on a five-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

Affectivity. Affectivity was measured with a 10-item international Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Short Form (I-PANAS-SF) (Thompson, 2007). The scale consists of five items that measure positive affectivity and five items that measure negative affectivity. Polish translation of items was taken from previous research (Wróbel et al., 2019). Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they usually feel each emotion on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (very slightly) to 5 (very much).

Social inhibition. A seven-item scale developed by Denollet (2005) was used to measure social inhibition. Items were translated into Polish following the back-translation procedure (Brislin, 1986). All items were measured by a five-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

Knowledge hiding. Four items from Rhee and Choi (2017) were used to measure knowledge hiding. All items were rated on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The back-translation procedure (Brislin, 1986) was used to translate items from English into Polish. The scale opened with the following statement: “Knowledge refers to facts, experiences and information. Please think of recent interactions with coworkers who requested knowledge from you and how you responded to them” (adapted from Rhee and Choi, 2017). To measure supervisor knowledge hiding, items for knowledge hiding were modified by changing the referent in three items from “I” to “my supervisor”. In this way, the respondents were supposed to assess their supervisor’s behavior rather than their own behavior. The fourth item – “I tried to hide innovative solutions and achievement” – was not modified and was not included in the scale for supervisor knowledge hiding because respondents would have had difficulty assessing whether supervisors hid innovative solutions and achievement. The scale opened with the following statement: “For a moment, visualize in your mind your supervisor. How does he/she usually behave upon receiving a request from you for any specific knowledge?” (adapted from Arain et al., 2019).

3.3 Statistical analyses

Partial least squares path modeling (PLS-PM) technique and SmartPLS 3.2.9 software (Ringle et al., 2015) was used to evaluate relationships between variables in this study. PLS-PM has been successfully applied in previous empirical research on relationships between personality traits and counterproductive knowledge behavior (Serenko and Choo, 2020). The use of PLS included the assessment of the measurement and structural model. All constructs in this study were reflective.

4. Results

4.1 The measurement model

The measurement model was assessed using reliability and validity tests. Reliabilities of the individual items was assessed based on outer loading value. Three items – DDF2, PA5, and SI2 (see Appendix) – with outer loading below 0.55 were deleted from the model, which contributes to an increase in average variance extracted (AVE). The further tests were conducted without the three deleted items. The loadings of five items were between 0.648 and 0.683, which is slightly below the recommended threshold of 0.7 (Hair et al., 2019). However, they were retained in the model to maintain the content validity of the scale, which is an acceptable approach (Serenko and Choo, 2020). Internal consistency reliability was measured using Cronbach’s alpha (α) and composite reliability (CR). Values of these two measures were above the recommended threshold of 0.7 (see Appendix). The AVE was calculated to assess the convergent validity. AVE values were between 0.574 and 0.796, which meant they exceeded the minimum acceptable value of 0.5 (Hair et al., 2019). Cross-loading of indicator, Fornell-Larcker criterion and the Heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) ratio of correlation were used to evaluate the discriminant validity. Cross-loading analysis revealed that all indicators were loaded higher on their assigned construct than on other constructs. Moreover, the square root of the AVE for each construct was greater than the correlation values (Table 1), which fulfilled the Fornell–Larcker criterion. Finally, the HTMT values were below the cut-off value of 0.90 (Henseler et al., 2015) (Table 1). In summary, the above results indicated good reliability and validity of the measurement tool.

4.2 The structural model

Assessing the structural model, the variance inflation factor (VIF), the coefficient of determination (R2), the redundancy measure Q2 and the statistical significance of the path coefficients were considered. The VIF values for all the latent variables were below 3, which means that collinearity was not an issue in this study. The adjusted R2 values were 0.323 for social inhibition and 0.342 for knowledge hiding (Figure 2). This means that the R2 values were moderate (Sanchez, 2013). The Q2 values were calculated using the blindfolding procedure and obtained values were larger than zero (for KH was 0.252 and for SI was 0.149). These values depicted the predictive relevance of this PLS model (Hair et al., 2019). Moreover, the following general model fit indices were calculated: SRMR = 0.078 (acceptable if <0.1) (Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003), d_ULS = 3.412, d_G = 0.742, chi-square = 21993.186, NFI = 0.779 and RMS theta = 0.124.

Bootstrapping with 10,000 re-sampling was used to test the statistical significance of the structural relationships. Table 2 shows t-statistics, p-values and bias corrected confidence intervals. As expected, there is a significant and positive relation between DIF and KH (β = 0.220, t = 4.171, p < 0.05), supporting H1. SI was positively and significantly influenced by DDF (β = 0.370, t = 7.751, p < 0.05) and NA (β = 0.292, t = 6.467, p < 0.05), supporting H2 and H3. Next, it was found that KH was also positively and significantly influenced by both SI (β = 0.112, t = 2.239, p < 0.05) and SKH (β = 0.461, t = 11.918, p < 0.05), supporting H4 and H5, respectively. In the case of H6, the results were not unequivocal. Based on the p-value, the results revealed that PA moderated the relationships between SKH and KH (β = −0.082, t = 2.080, p < 0.05). The results revealed that, at higher PA, SKH was found to have weaker impact on KH. These results suggest support for H6. However, the bootstrap confidence interval for the moderating effect was [−0.147; 0.006], and hence contained a zero. Therefore, based on the confidence interval, which is a more conservative approach than an approach based on p-value, H6 was not supported.

Additionally, it was investigated whether DDF and NA had direct and indirect effect on KH. Both of those constructs did not have direct effects on KH, but indirect effects, via social inhibition, were significant (Table 3). The indirect effect of DDF on KH (β = 0.041, t = 2.348, p < 0.05) was stronger than the indirect effect of NA on KH (β = 0.033, t = 2.127, p < 0.05).

5. Discussion and implications

5.1 Discussion

This study examined how personality traits affected knowledge hiding. It showed that difficulty identifying feelings and social inhibition are positively related to knowledge hiding (H1 and H4). These findings are consistent with previous studies, which suggested that a deficit in identifying own emotional states decreases empathy (Moriguchi et al., 2007) and altruism in social decisions (FeldmanHall et al., 2013). Other studies have revealed that socially inhibited individuals avoid socializing, are shy and are reluctant to conduct conversations (Aboutorab and Raziyeh, 2012; Duijndam et al., 2021), which may strengthen knowledge hiding behavior. The behavior of alexithymics and social inhibited individuals might be explained by COR. As argued in Section 2.2, because of strong sense of psychological and social resources deficiencies, they might choose to protect their knowledge resources rather than risk investing in new resources.

This study confirmed that other dimension of alexithymia, difficulty describing feeling, has a significant impact on social inhibition (H2). Hence, the inability to describe and interpret one’s own emotions may contribute to social misunderstandings and disappointments, disrupting social relationships and increasing the tendency to avoid relationships with other people. This finding is in line with empirical research conducted by Scheerer et al. (2021), who found that alexithymia is negatively related with social competence, including verbal conversation skills. Furthermore, this study argued that high-NA individuals, because of their generally bad mood and negative attitude towards others, have a tendency to avoid social interactions. The results of this study supported this prediction (H3). These results are consistent with previous suggestions that high-NA individuals perceive other people as more hostile and threatening (Thoresen et al., 2003), perceive tasks more negatively and demonstrate a greater frequency of withdrawal behaviors at work (Necowitz and Roznowski, 1994). However, contrary to previous studies (Ma and Zhang, 2021; Peng et al., 2020), the present indicated that negative affectivity is not directly related to knowledge hiding, but indirectly, via social inhibition.

The study also revealed that supervisor knowledge hiding is positively related to subordinate’s knowledge hiding from co-workers (H5). This result can be seen as empirical evidence supporting a theory of behavioral contagion and the suggestions presented in previous studies. For example, Offergelt et al. (2019) found that if a leader sends direct or indirect signals that knowledge hiding is acceptable, employees are more likely to engage in knowledge hiding. Moreover, the present study shows that positive affectivity can moderate the relationship between supervisor knowledge hiding and subordinate’s knowledge hiding from co-workers (H6). Hence, positive affectivity can be a personal resource that helps an employee cope with adverse organizational situations and mitigates his/her retaliatory behavior. This finding is in line with previous studies, which found that positive affectivity moderates the effect of psychological contract breaches (Jahanzeb et al., 2020) and competitive psychological climate (Han et al., 2020) on knowledge hiding.

5.2 Theoretical implications

First, this study contributes to the knowledge management literature by investigating the impact of personality traits on individual knowledge hiding. Previous studies focused on Big Five personality traits or the dark triad in the context of knowledge hiding (Hamza et al., 2021; Pan et al., 2018). This is the first study to show that alexithymia, particularly difficulty identifying feelings, and social inhibition are significant predictors of knowledge hiding (H1 and H4). Moreover, the present study uses COR to explain why alexithymics and social inhibited individuals choose to hide their knowledge. These findings develop our understanding of how personality traits drive counterproductive knowledge behavior. Understanding why employees hide knowledge is a prerequisite for achieving effective knowledge management in an organization (Pan et al., 2018).

Second, this study contributes to a better understanding of the formation of knowledge hiding in an organization by exploring the role of social inhibition in the relation between negative affectivity and knowledge hiding and between difficulty describing feeling and knowledge hiding. Past research has demonstrated the impact of a negative emotion state on counterproductive work behavior (Yang and Diefendorff, 2009), including employee knowledge hiding behavior (Ma and Zhang, 2021; Peng et al., 2020). The present study extends this by showing that negative affectivity is not directly related to knowledge hiding, but has a strong effect on social inhibition (H3), which in turn is related to knowledge hiding (H4). Similarly, difficulty describing feeling is significantly related to social inhibition (H2), but it is not directly related to knowledge hiding. As a bootstrap test confirmed (Table 3), these indirect effects of negative affectivity and difficulty describing feeling on knowledge hiding are significant. Hence, the present study indicates that employees with Type D personality – that is, a personality characterized by a combination of NA and social inhibition – may have a tendency to hide knowledge. Previous studies have linked the Type D personality to a variety of adverse mental states, including depression and low levels of self-esteem (Denollet, 2005). However, the impact of Type D personality on knowledge hiding has not been studied before.

Third, this study reveals that different dimensions of alexithymia might have a direct or indirect effect on knowledge hiding. In particular, difficulty identifying feeling has a direct effect on knowledge hiding (H1), whereas difficulty describing feeling has an indirect effect, via social inhibition. This finding confirms that alexithymia is a multidimensional construct and its dimensions in relation to different employee behaviors should be considered separately. This finding is partly in line with Meganck et al.’s (2009) findings that three subscales of alexithymia have different influence on interpersonal style.

Fourth, this study highlights the huge impact of supervisor knowledge hiding on individual knowledge hiding (H5). Previous studies have indicated that employees reciprocate knowledge hiding (Serenko and Bontis, 2016) and co-worker knowledge sabotage may be contagious workplace behavior (Serenko and Choo, 2020). However, this study sought to answer the question of whether the fact that the supervisor hides knowledge from his/her subordinate makes the subordinate tend to hide knowledge from his/her co-workers. As discussed in Section 2.10, this relationship might be explained by a conversation of resource theory and a theory of behavioral contagion. Overall, this study suggests that an employee from whom supervisors used to hide knowledge has a tendency to mimic this behavior toward co-workers and protect his/her knowledge resources from co-workers.

Fifth, this study contributes to knowledge management by identifying and examining a moderator in the relationship between supervisor knowledge hiding and individual knowledge hiding. It turned out that positive affectivity might be a weak moderator of this relationship (H6). Positive affectivity may serve as a buffer that mitigates the reaction to unpleasant experiences, including supervisor knowledge hiding.

5.3 Managerial implications

First, it is important for managers to be aware that certain personality traits, including alexithymia, social inhibition and negative affectivity, may predispose employees to hide knowledge. Hence, managers who want to reduce knowledge hiding should pay special attention to the personal characteristics of job applicants and not hire individuals with those undesirable traits. Instruments and scales to measure alexithymia, social inhibition and negative affectivity (such as TAS-20) may be used at the recruitment stage to select appropriate candidates. Because a job applicant may disguise his/her true personality, it is also worth getting to know the opinion of previous employers and co-workers about the personality of the job applicant. Moreover, an employer can use a trial period of employment to observe the employee in terms of his or her employee’s personality traits.

Second, to reduce knowledge hiding, it may be also beneficial for an organization to identify those hired employees who have high levels of alexithymia, social inhibition and negative affectivity. For those employees, managers can offer special trainings to overcome their psychological and social deficiencies, including rebuilding self-confidence, coping with negative emotions and developing social skills. However, these trainings might only be partially effective, as alexithymia, social inhibition and negative affectivity are perceived as rather stable personal traits. Another solution is to allocate those employees to positions that are more appropriate for their personality traits.

Third, managers should realize that supervisor knowledge hiding leads to subordinate knowledge hiding. Being frequent witnesses of knowledge hiding by superiors, subordinates may have a false belief that such behavior is accepted in the organization and, consequently, they may imitate it. Therefore, managers should clearly define what behaviors are desired and rewarded in the organization and take steps to modify the behavior of supervisors who hide knowledge. Organizations can use employee surveys in which subordinates assess their supervisors in terms of hiding knowledge. For supervisors who would be identified as hiding knowledge, appropriate training sessions on knowledge sharing and workplace relations could be offered.

5.4 Limitations and future research

This study has certain limitations that can be overcome in future research. First, some limitations arise from the way the data were collected. The study used a paid online panel, which may have an impact on representativeness and quality responses. The online nature of data collection limits participation to those who have access to the Internet. Furthermore, panelists may have filled out the survey too quickly, dishonestly and without due attention, as they were more interested in receiving points and then prizes than in providing high-quality responses. To increase the quality of data in future studies, some “check-items” can be included in the survey (Achnak and Vantilborgh, 2021). Additionally, the study used self-reported measures to evaluate personality traits. This approach has been commonly used in previous research on alexithymia, social inhibition and negative affectivity (Denollet, 2005; Mattila et al., 2007). However, respondents might underrate the intensity of these traits in themselves, realizing that these traits are not socially desirable. In future research, managers and co-workers could also assess the personal traits of an employee.

Second, the perception of alexithymia as a three-dimensional construct is a popular view in the literature (Bagby et al., 1994). The present study only investigated two of three dimensions of alexithymia – difficulty identifying feelings (DIF) and difficulty describing feelings (DDF) – because, as the literature review indicated, these two dimensions were expected to have significant relations with knowledge hiding. However, future research may find a theoretical framework and empirical evidence for the relationship between the third dimensions of alexithymia, that is, externally orientated thinking style, and knowledge hiding.

Third, this study used a short, four-item scale for knowledge hiding, proposed by Rhee and Choi (2017). However, in empirical studies on hiding knowledge, many researchers use the 12-item scale developed by Connelly et al. (2012), which measures three dimensions of knowledge hiding, including evasive hiding, playing dumb and rationalized hiding. Thus, future studies can investigate the separate effects of alexithymia, social inhibition and negative affectivity on these three different dimensions of knowledge hiding.

Fourth, this study suggests that employees’ positive affectivity moderates the relationship between supervisor knowledge hiding and individual knowledge hiding. However, this moderating effect is weak, so it needs to be confirmed in future studies. Future studies may also examine the moderating effect of other positive personality traits and attitudes that can weaken the effect of supervisor knowledge hiding on individual knowledge hiding, including emotional resilience, self-confidence and proactive personality.

Finally, this study has not taken into account organizational conditions that may affect the relationship between the employee’s personality and hiding knowledge. These conditions can activate or deactivate workers’ behaviors that are a manifestation of their personality traits, which is in line with the trait activation theory (Tett and Burnett, 2003). Therefore, the inclusion of organizational factors as moderators in the research model, such as organizational culture, would be a valuable extension of this study.

6. Conclusion

This study extends previous research on relationships between personality traits and counterproductive knowledge behavior. Based on conservation of resources theory, a model of relationships between alexithymia, social inhibition, affectivity and knowledge hiding was developed and tested empirically. From the theoretical perspective, this study investigates previously unexplored relationships and enables a better understanding of the impact of adverse employees’ personality traits and supervisor attitude on employees’ knowledge hiding. From the managerial perspective, it provides suggestions how to reduce knowledge hiding behaviors. The results of this study are expected to trigger further research on relationships between alexithymia, social inhibition, affectivity and different knowledge behaviors.

Figures

The proposed model

Figure 1

The proposed model

The structural model

Figure 2

The structural model

Descriptive statistics, construct correlations and results for discriminant validity

Variables Mean SD DIF DDF NA PA SI SKH KH
DIF 2.62 0.94 0.758 0.869 0.623 0.193 0.424 0.370 0.428
DDF 2.77 0.90 0.704** 0.766 0.574 0.187 0.594 0.294 0.377
NA 2.72 0.76 0.530** 0.469 0.764 0.233 0.520 0.282 0.238
PA 3.50 0.67 −0.183** −0.160** −0.114* 0.771 0.355 0.051 0.100
SI 2.82 0.93 0.386** 0.511** 0.464** −0.296 0.800 0.200 0.274
SKH 2.56 1.12 0.324** 0.235** 0.240** −0.008 0.187** 0.892 0.613
KH 2.27 1.00 0.381** 0.299** 0.207** −0.096 0.257** 0.532** 0.853
Notes:
*

p < 0.01;

**

p < 0.001; The italic diagonal elements represent the square root of AVE; Values below the diagonal are the construct correlations; Values above the diagonal are the HTMT values; SD – Standard deviation; Scales: DIF – Difficulty Identifying Feeling; DDF – Difficulty Describing Feeling; NA – Negative Affectivity; PA – Positive Affectivity; SKH – Supervisor Knowledge Hiding; KH – Knowledge Hiding

Structural model results

Hypothesis Suggested effect Path coefficient t-value p-value 95% BCa CI Support
H1. Difficulty Identifying Feeling → Knowledge Hiding + 0.220 4.171 0.000 [0.114; 0.320] Sig. Yes
H2. Difficulty Describing Feeling → Social Inhibition + 0.370 7.751 0.000 [0.271; 0.458] Sig. Yes
H3. Negative Affectivity → Social Inhibition + 0.292 6.467 0.000 [0.203; 0.380] Sig. Yes
H4. Social Inhibition → Knowledge Hiding + 0.112 2.239 0.017 [0.024; 0.208] Sig. Yes
H5. Supervisor Knowledge Hiding → Knowledge Hiding + 0.461 11.918 0.000 [0.382; 0.534] Sig. Yes
H6. Supervisor Knowledge Hiding*Positive Affectivity → Knowledge Hiding −0.082 2.080 0.038 [−0.147; 0.006] Nsig. Yes
Notes:

BCa CI = bias corrected confidence interval; Sig. = a significant direct effect at 0.05; Nsig. = a non-significant direct effect at 0.05; Bootstrapping based on n = 10,000 subsamples

Bootstrap test of direct, indirect and total effects

Path Direct
effect
95% BCa CI Indirect
effect
95% BCa CI Total
effect
95% BCa CI
Difficulty Describing Feeling → Social Inhibition → Knowledge Hiding 0.024 [−0.077;0.131] Nsig. 0.041 [0.010;0.080] Sig. 0.066 [−0.031;0.167] Nsig.
Negative Affectivity → Social Inhibition → Knowledge Hiding −0.079 [−0.174;0.010] Nsig. 0.033 [0.007;0.069] Sig. −0.047 [−0.136;0.040] Nsig.
Notes:

BCa CI = bias corrected confidence interval; Sig. = a significant direct effect at 0.05

Appendix. Items

Alexithymia (Bagby et al., 1994)

  1. Difficulty Identifying Feelings (α = 0.875, CR = 0.903, AVE = 0.574)

    • DIF1. I am often confused about what emotion I am feeling.

    • DIF2. I have physical sensations that even doctors don’t understand.

    • DIF3. When I am upset, I don’t know if I am sad, frightened, or angry.

    • DIF4. I am often puzzled by sensations in my body.

    • DIF5. I have feelings that I can’t quite identify.

    • DIF6. I don’t know what’s going on inside me.

    • DIF7. I often don’t know why I am angry.

  2. Difficulty Describing Feelings (α = 0.764, CR = 0.849, AVE = 0.586)

    • DDF1. It is difficult for me to find the right words for my feelings.

    • DDF2. I am able to describe my feelings easily. (R)*

    • DDF3. I find it hard to describe how I feel about people.

    • DDF4. People tell me to describe my feelings more.

    • DDF5. It is difficult for me to reveal my innermost feelings, even to close friends.

Affectivity (Thompson, 2007)

To what extent do you usually feel the following emotions:

  1. Negative Affectivity (α = 0.821, CR = 0.874, AVE = 0.583)

    • NA1. afraid

    • NA2. ashamed

    • NA3. hostile

    • NA4. nervous

    • NA5. upset

  2. Positive affectivity (α = 0.786, CR = 0.854, AVE = 0.595)

    • PA1. active

    • PA2. alert

    • PA3. attentive

    • PA4. determined

    • PA5. inspired*

  3. Social Inhibition (Denollet, 2005) (α = 0.887, CR = 0.914, AVE = 0.640)

    • SI1. I make contact easily when I meet people. (R)

    • SI2. I often talk to strangers. (R)*

    • SI3. I often feel inhibited in social interactions.

    • SI4. I find it hard to start a conversation.

    • SI5. I am a closed kind of person.

    • SI6. I would rather keep other people at a distance.

    • SI7. When socializing, I don’t find the right things to talk about.

  4. Knowledge hiding (Rhee and Choi, 2017) (α = 0.869, CR = 0.921, AVE = 0.796)

    • KH1. I agreed to help him/her but never really intended to.

    • KH2. I pretended that I did not know the information.

    • KH3. I said that I did not know even though I did.

    • KH4. I tried to hide innovative solutions and achievement.

  5. Supervisor knowledge hiding (adapted from Rhee and Choi, 2017) (α = 0.871, CR = 0.913, AVE = 0.727)

    • SKH1. My supervisor agrees to help him/her but never really intends to.

    • SKH2. My supervisor pretends that he/she does not know the information.

    • SKH3. He/she says that he/she does not know even though he/she does.

Note: R – reverse coded; * – an item removed after scale validation.

References

Abbas, M., Raja, U., Darr, W. and Bouckenooghe, D. (2014), “Combined effects of perceived politics and psychological Capital on job satisfaction, turnover intentions, and performance”, Journal of Management, Vol. 40 No. 7, pp. 1813-1830.

Aboutorab, T. and Raziyeh, B.D. (2012), “Examining the quality of trust and social withdrawal among university students”, Social Development and Welfare Planning, Vol. 4 No. 11, pp. 131-182.

Achnak, S. and Vantilborgh, T. (2021), “Do individuals combine different coping strategies to manage their stress in the aftermath of psychological contract breach over time? A longitudinal study”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 131, p. 103651.

Agarwal, U.A., Avey, J. and Wu, K. (2021), “How and when abusive supervision influences knowledge hiding behavior: evidence from India”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 26 No. 1, available at: https://doi.org/10.1108/JKM-10-2020-0789

Agyemang, F.G., Dzandu, M.D. and Boateng, H. (2016), “Knowledge sharing among teachers: the role of the big five personality traits”, VINE Journal of Information and Knowledge Management Systems, Vol. 46 No. 1, pp. 64-84.

Anand, A., Offergelt, F. and Anand, P. (2021), “Knowledge hiding – a systematic review and research agenda”, Journal of Knowledge Management, available at: http://www.emerald.com/insight/content/doi/10.1108/JKM-04-2021-0336/full/html

Anand, P. and Hassan, Y. (2019), “Knowledge hiding in organizations: everything that managers need to know”, Development and Learning in Organizations: An International Journal, Vol. 33 No. 6, pp. 12-15.

Arain, G.A., Bhatti, Z.A., Hameed, I. and Fang, Y.-H. (2019), “Top-down knowledge hiding and innovative work behavior (IWB): a three-way moderated-mediation analysis of self-efficacy and local/foreign status”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 24 No. 2, pp. 127-149.

Arshad, R. and Ismail, I.R. (2018), “Workplace incivility and knowledge hiding behavior: does personality matter?”, Journal of Organizational Effectiveness: People and Performance, Vol. 5 No. 3, pp. 278-288.

Aslan, G., Bakan, A.B. and Yildiz, M. (2021), “An investigation of the relationship between alexithymia and empathy tendency in university students receiving health education”, Perspectives in Psychiatric Care, Vol. 57 No. 2, pp. 709-716.

Ayub, A., Ajmal, T., Iqbal, S., Ghazanfar, S., Anwaar, M. and Ishaq, M. (2021), “Abusive supervision and knowledge hiding in service organizations: exploring the boundary conditions”, International Journal of Conflict Management, Vol. 32 No. 5, pp. 725-746.

Bagby, R.M., Parker, J.D.A. and Taylor, G.J. (1994), “The twenty-item Toronto alexithymia scale – I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure”, Journal of Psychosomatic Research, Vol. 38 No. 1, pp. 23-32.

Ball, G.A., Trevino, L.K. and Sims, H.P. (1993), “Justice and organizational punishment: attitudinal outcomes of disciplinary events”, Social Justice Research, Vol. 6 No. 1, pp. 39-67.

Bandura, A. (1999), “Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities”, Personality and Social Psychology Review, Vol. 3 No. 3, pp. 193-209.

Banzhaf, C., Hoffmann, F., Kanske, P., Fan, Y., Walter, H., Spengler, S. and Schreiter, S. (2018), “Interacting and dissociable effects of alexithymia and depression on empathy”, Psychiatry Research, Vol. 270, pp. 631-638.

Belschak, F.D., Den Hartog, D.N. and De Hoogh, A.H.B. (2018), “Angels and demons: the effect of ethical leadership on machiavellian employees’ work behaviors”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 9, p. 1082.

Bengtsgard, K. and Bohlin, G. (2001), “Social inhibition and overfriendliness: two-year follow-up and observational validation”, Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, Vol. 30 No. 3, pp. 364-375.

Besharat, M.A. (2010), “Relationship of alexithymia with coping styles and interpersonal problems”, Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 5, pp. 614-618.

Bowling, N.A., Hendricks, E.A. and Wagner, S.H. (2008), “Positive and negative affectivity and facet satisfaction: a Meta-analysis”, Journal of Business and Psychology, Vol. 23 Nos 3/4, pp. 115-125.

Brislin, R.W. (1986), “The wording and translation of research instruments”, in Lonner, W.J. and Berry, J.W. (Eds), Field Methods in Cross-Cultural Research, Sage Publications, Inc, Thousand Oaks, CA, pp. 137-164.

Caputo, F., Magni, D., Papa, A. and Corsi, C. (2021), “Knowledge hiding in socioeconomic settings: matching organizational and environmental antecedents”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 135, pp. 19-27.

Černe, M., Nerstad, C.G.L., Dysvik, A. and Škerlavaj, M. (2014), “What goes around comes around: knowledge hiding, perceived motivational climate, and creativity”, Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 57 No. 1, pp. 172-192.

Chen, Y., Luo, H., Chen, J. and Guo, Y. (2022), “Building data-driven dynamic capabilities to arrest knowledge hiding: a knowledge management perspective”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 139, pp. 1138-1154.

Cillo, V., Rialti, R., Bertoldi, B. and Ciampi, F. (2019), “Knowledge management and open innovation in Agri-food crowdfunding”, British Food Journal, Vol. 121 No. 2, pp. 242-258.

Cillo, V., Gregori, G.L., Daniele, L.M., Caputo, F. and Bitbol-Saba, N. (2021), “Rethinking companies’ culture through knowledge management lens during industry 5.0 transition”, Journal of Knowledge Management, available at: https://doi.org/10.1108/JKM-09-2021-0718

Connelly, C.E. and Zweig, D. (2015), “How perpetrators and targets construe knowledge hiding in organizations”, European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 24 No. 3, pp. 479-489.

Connelly, C.E., Zweig, D., Webster, J. and Trougakos, J.P. (2012), “Knowledge hiding in organizations”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 33 No. 1, pp. 64-88.

Cook, R., Brewer, R., Shah, P. and Bird, G. (2013), “Alexithymia, not autism, predicts poor recognition of emotional facial expressions”, Psychological Science, Vol. 24 No. 5, pp. 723-732.

Cropanzano, R. and Mitchell, M.S. (2005), “Social exchange theory: an interdisciplinary review”, Journal of Management, Vol. 31 No. 6, pp. 874-900.

de Moor, E.L., Denollet, J. and Laceulle, O.M. (2018), “Social inhibition, sense of belonging and vulnerability to internalizing problems”, Journal of Affective Disorders, Vol. 225, pp. 207-213.

Decety, J., Bartal, I.B.-A., Uzefovsky, F. and Knafo-Noam, A. (2016), “Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species”, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, Vol. 371 No. 1686, p. 20150077.

Denollet, J. (2005), “DS14: standard assessment of negative affectivity, social inhibition, and type D personality”, Psychosomatic Medicine, Vol. 67 No. 1, pp. 89-97.

Denollet, J. and Duijndam, S. (2019), “The multidimensional nature of adult social inhibition: inhibition, sensitivity and withdrawal facets of the SIQ15”, Journal of Affective Disorders, Vol. 245, pp. 569-579.

Dimant, E. (2019), “Contagion of pro- and anti-social behavior among peers and the role of social proximity”, Journal of Economic Psychology, Vol. 73, pp. 66-88.

Don, B., Girme, Y. and Hammond, M. (2019), “Low self-esteem predicts indirect support seeking and its relationship consequences in intimate relationships”, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 45 No. 7, pp. 1028-1041.

Duijndam, S., Karreman, A., Denollet, J. and Kupper, N. (2021), “Situation selection and modification in social inhibition: a person-centered approach”, Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, Vol. 1, pp. 1-14.

Eder, P. and Eisenberger, R. (2008), “Perceived organizational support: reducing the negative influence of coworker withdrawal behavior”, Journal of Management, Vol. 34 No. 1, pp. 55-68.

FeldmanHall, O., Dalgleish, T. and Mobbs, D. (2013), “Alexithymia decreases altruism in real social decisions”, Cortex, Vol. 49 No. 3, pp. 899-904.

Franz, M., Popp, K., Schaefer, R., Sitte, W., Schneider, C., Hardt, J., Decker, O., et al. (2008), “Alexithymia in the German general population”, Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, Vol. 43 No. 1, pp. 54-62.

Geofroy, Z. and Evans, M.M. (2017), “Are emotionally intelligent employees less likely to hide their knowledge?”, Knowledge and Process Management, Vol. 24 No. 2, pp. 81-95.

Gest, S.D. (1997), “Behavioral inhibition: stability and associations with adaptation from childhood to early adulthood”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 72 No. 2, pp. 467-475.

Gino, F., Ayal, S. and Ariely, D. (2009), “Contagion and differentiation in unethical behavior: the effect of one bad apple on the barrel”, Psychological Science, Vol. 20 No. 3, pp. 393-398.

Goerlich, K.S. (2018), “The multifaceted nature of alexithymia – a neuroscientific perspective”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 9, available at: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01614

Goerlich-Dobre, K.S., Bruce, L., Martens, S., Aleman, A. and Hooker, C.I. (2014), “Distinct associations of insula and cingulate volume with the cognitive and affective dimensions of alexithymia”, Neuropsychologia, Vol. 53, pp. 284-292.

Gouldner, A.W. (1960), “The norm of reciprocity: a preliminary statement”, American Sociological Review, Vol. 25 No. 2, pp. 161-178.

Guttman, H. and Laporte, L. (2002), “Alexithymia, empathy, and psychological symptoms in a family context”, Comprehensive Psychiatry, Vol. 43 No. 6, pp. 448-455.

Hair, J.F., Risher, J.J., Sarstedt, M. and Ringle, C.M. (2019), “When to use and how to report the results of PLS-SEM”, European Business Review, Vol. 31 No. 1, pp. 2-24.

Hamza, M.A., Rehman, S., Sarwar, A. and Choudhary, K.N. (2021), “Is knowledge a tenement? The mediating role of team member exchange over the relationship of big five personality traits and knowledge-hiding behavior”, VINE Journal of Information and Knowledge Management Systems, available at: https://doi.org/10.1108/VJIKMS-05-2020-0084

Han, M.S., Masood, K., Cudjoe, D. and Wang, Y. (2020), “Knowledge hiding as the dark side of competitive psychological climate”, Leadership & Organization Development Journal, Vol. 42 No. 2, pp. 195-207.

Harris, M.A. and Orth, U. (2020), “The link between self-esteem and social relationships: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 119 No. 6, pp. 1459-1477.

Heng, C.S., Lin, Z., Xu, X., Zhang, Y. and Zhao, Y. (2019), “Human flesh search: what did we find?”, Information & Management, Vol. 56 No. 4, pp. 476-492.

Henseler, J., Ringle, C.M. and Sarstedt, M. (2015), “A new criterion for assessing discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling”, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 43 No. 1, pp. 115-135.

Hernaus, T., Cerne, M., Connelly, C., Poloski Vokic, N. and Škerlavaj, M. (2019), “Evasive knowledge hiding in academia: when competitive individuals are asked to collaborate”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 23 No. 4, pp. 597-618.

Hobfoll, S.E. (1989), “Conservation of resources: a new attempt at conceptualizing stress”, American Psychologist, Vol. 44 No. 3, pp. 513-524.

Hobfoll, S.E. (2001), “The influence of culture, community, and the nested-self in the stress process: advancing conservation of resources theory”, Applied Psychology, Vol. 50 No. 3, pp. 337-421.

Hobfoll, S.E., Halbesleben, J., Neveu, J.-P. and Westman, M. (2018), “Conservation of resources in the organizational context: the reality of resources and their consequences”, Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, Vol. 5 No. 1, pp. 103-128.

Jahanzeb, S., Clercq, D.D. and Fatima, T. (2020), “Bridging the breach: using positive affectivity to overcome knowledge hiding after contract breaches”, The Journal of Psychology, Vol. 154 No. 3, pp. 249-272.

Jiang, Z. and Hu, X. (2016), “Knowledge sharing and life satisfaction: the roles of colleague relationships and gender”, Social Indicators Research, Vol. 126 No. 1, pp. 379-394.

Karim, D.N. (2020), “Effect of dark personalities on knowledge hiding behaviour at higher education institutions”, Journal of Information & Knowledge Management, Vol. 19 No. 04, p. 2050031.

Kmieciak, R. (2021), “Knowledge-withholding behaviours among IT specialists: the roles of job insecurity, work overload and supervisor support”, Journal of Management & Organization, pp. 1-17.

Koay, K.Y. and Lim, P.K. (2021), “Ethical leadership and knowledge hiding: testing the mediating and moderating mechanisms”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 26 No. 3, available at: https://doi.org/10.1108/JKM-02-2021-0091

Koay, K.Y., Sandhu, M.S., Tjiptono, F. and Watabe, M. (2020), “Understanding employees’ knowledge hiding behaviour: the moderating role of market culture”, Behaviour & Information Technology, Vol. 1, pp. 1-18.

Liang, H., Lin, K.-Y., Zhang, S. and Su, Y. (2018), “The impact of coworkers’ safety violations on an individual worker: a social contagion effect within the construction crew”, International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, Vol. 15 No. 4, p. 773.

Liao, Z., Liu, Y. and Li, M. (2020), “Is environmental innovation contagious? A study on the mechanism of individual firms’ environmental innovation affecting the industry”, Sustainable Development, Vol. 28 No. 6, pp. 1787-1795.

Liu, F., Lu, Y. and Wang, P. (2020), “Why knowledge sharing in scientific research teams is difficult to sustain: an interpretation from the interactive perspective of knowledge hiding behavior”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 11, available at: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.537833

Ma, B. and Zhang, J. (2021), “Are overqualified individuals hiding knowledge: the mediating role of negative emotion state”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 26 No. 3, available at: https://doi.org/10.1108/JKM-01-2021-0022

Ma, L., Zhang, X. and Ding, X. (2020), “Enterprise social media usage and knowledge hiding: a motivation theory perspective”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 24 No. 9, pp. 2149-2169.

Majid, S., Idio, C.D., Liang, S. and Zhang, W. (2015), “Preferences and motivating factors for knowledge sharing by students”, Journal of Information & Knowledge Management, Vol. 14 No. 01.

Mattila, A.K., Salminen, J.K., Nummi, T. and Joukamaa, M. (2006), “Age is strongly associated with alexithymia in the general population”, Journal of Psychosomatic Research, Vol. 61 No. 5, pp. 629-635.

Mattila, A.K., Ahola, K., Honkonen, T., Salminen, J.K., Huhtala, H. and Joukamaa, M. (2007), “Alexithymia and occupational burnout are strongly associated in working population”, Journal of Psychosomatic Research, Vol. 62 No. 6, pp. 657-665.

Meganck, R., Vanheule, S., Inslegers, R. and Desmet, M. (2009), “Alexithymia and interpersonal problems: a study of natural language use”, Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 47 No. 8, pp. 990-995.

Michailova, S. and Husted, K. (2003), “Knowledge-sharing hostility in Russian firms”, California Management Review, Vol. 45 No. 3, pp. 59-77.

Michna, A. and Kmieciak, R. (2020), “Open-mindedness culture, knowledge-sharing, financial performance, and industry 4.0 in SMEs”, Sustainability, Vol. 12 No. 21, p. 9041.

Morie, K.P., Yip, S.W., Nich, C., Hunkele, K., Carroll, K.M. and Potenza, M.N. (2016), “Alexithymia and addiction: a review and preliminary data suggesting neurobiological links to reward/loss processing”, Current Addiction Reports, Vol. 3 No. 2, pp. 239-248.

Moriguchi, Y., Decety, J., Ohnishi, T., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto, K. and Matsuda, H. (2007), “Empathy and judging other’s pain: an fMRI study of alexithymia”, Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 17 No. 9, pp. 2223-2234.

Naquin, S.S. and Holton, E.F. (2002), “The effects of personality, affectivity, and work commitment on motivation to improve work through learning”, Human Resource Development Quarterly, Vol. 13 No. 4, pp. 357-376.

Necowitz, L.B. and Roznowski, M. (1994), “Negative affectivity and job satisfaction: cognitive processes underlying the relationship and effects on employee behaviors”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 45 No. 3, pp. 270-294.

Obrenovic, B., Jianguo, D., Tsoy, D., Obrenovic, S., Khan, M.A.S. and Anwar, F. (2020), “The enjoyment of knowledge sharing: impact of altruism on tacit knowledge-sharing behavior”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 11, available at: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01496

Offergelt, F., Spörrle, M., Moser, K. and Shaw, J.D. (2019), “Leader-signaled knowledge hiding: effects on employees’ job attitudes and empowerment”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 40 No. 7, pp. 819-833.

Ogunlade, J.O. (1979), “Personality characteristics related to susceptibility to behavioral contagion”, Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, Vol. 7 No. 2, pp. 205-208.

Oktary, D., Marjohan, M. and Syahniar, S. (2019), “The effects of self-confidence and social support of parents on interpersonal communication of students”, Journal of Educational and Learning Studies, Vol. 2 No. 1, pp. 5-11.

Palma-Álvarez, R.F., Ros-Cucurull, E., Daigre, C., Perea-Ortueta, M., Martínez-Luna, N., Serrano-Pérez, P. and Sorribes-Puertas, M. (2021), “Is alexithymia related to retention and relapses in patients with substance use disorders?: A one year follow-up study”, Addictive Behaviors, Vol. 113, p. 106681.

Pan, W., Zhang, Q., Teo, T.S.H. and Lim, V.K.G. (2018), “The dark triad and knowledge hiding”, International Journal of Information Management, Vol. 42, pp. 36-48.

Pearson, J.C., Child, J.T., DeGreeff, B.L., Semlak, J.L. and Burnett, A. (2011), “The influence of biological sex, self-esteem, and communication apprehension on unwillingness to communicate”, Atlantic Journal of Communication, Vol. 19 No. 4, pp. 216-227.

Peng, H., Bell, C. and Li, Y. (2020), “How and when intragroup relationship conflict leads to knowledge hiding: the roles of envy and trait competitiveness”, International Journal of Conflict Management, Vol. 32 No. 3, pp. 383-406.

Penney, L.M. and Spector, P.E. (2005), “Job stress, incivility, and counterproductive work behavior (CWB): the moderating role of negative affectivity”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 26 No. 7, pp. 777-796.

Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Lee, J.-Y. and Podsakoff, N.P. (2003), “Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 88 No. 5, pp. 879-903.

Radaelli, G., Lettieri, E., Mura, M. and Spiller, N. (2014), “Knowledge sharing and innovative work behaviour in healthcare: a micro-level investigation of direct and indirect effects”, Creativity and Innovation Management, Vol. 23 No. 4, pp. 400-414.

Rhee, Y.W. and Choi, J.N. (2017), “Knowledge management behavior and individual creativity: goal orientations as antecedents and in-group social status as moderating contingency”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 38 No. 6, pp. 813-832.

Riege, A. (2005), “Three‐dozen knowledge‐sharing barriers managers must consider”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 9 No. 3, pp. 18-35.

Ringle, C.M. Wende, S. and Becker, J.-M. (2015), SmartPLS 3, SmartPLS GmbH, Boenningstedt, available at: www.smartpls.com

Robinson, S.L., Wang, W. and Kiewitz, C. (2014), “Coworkers behaving badly: the impact of coworker deviant behavior upon individual employees”, Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, Vol. 1 No. 1, pp. 123-143.

Rossi, M., Festa, G., Papa, A., Kolte, A. and Piccolo, R. (2020), “Knowledge management behaviors in venture Capital crossroads: a comparison between IVC and CVC ambidexterity”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 24 No. 10, pp. 2431-2454.

Samnani, A.-K., Salamon, S.D. and Singh, P. (2014), “Negative affect and counterproductive workplace behavior: the moderating role of moral disengagement and gender”, Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 119 No. 2, pp. 235-244.

Sanchez, G. (2013), PLS Path Modeling with R, Trowchez Editions, Berkeley.

Santoro, G., Thrassou, A., Bresciani, S. and Giudice, M.D. (2021), “Do knowledge management and dynamic capabilities affect ambidextrous entrepreneurial intensity and firms’ performance?”, IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management, Vol. 68 No. 2, pp. 378-386.

Scheerer, N.E., Boucher, T.Q. and Iarocci, G. (2021), “Alexithymia is related to poor social competence in autistic and nonautistic children”, Autism Research, Vol. 14 No. 6, pp. 1252-1259.

Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H. and Müller, H. (2003), “Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures”, Methods of Psychological Research, Vol. 8 No. 2, pp. 23-74.

Ścigała, D.K., Zdankiewicz-Ścigała, E., Bedyńska, S. and Kokoszka, A. (2020), “Psychometric properties and configural invariance of the Polish – language version of the 20-item toronto alexithymia scale in non-clinical and alcohol addict persons”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 11, available at: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01241

Serenko, A. and Bontis, N. (2016), “Understanding counterproductive knowledge behavior: antecedents and consequences of intra-organizational knowledge hiding”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 20 No. 6, pp. 1199-1224.

Serenko, A. and Choo, C.W. (2020), “Knowledge sabotage as an extreme form of counterproductive knowledge behavior: the role of narcissism, machiavellianism, psychopathy, and competitiveness”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 24 No. 9, pp. 2299-2325.

Siachou, E., Trichina, E., Papasolomou, I. and Sakka, G. (2021), “Why do employees hide their knowledge and what are the consequences? A systematic literature review”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 135, pp. 195-213.

Sifneos, P.E. (1973), “The prevalence of ‘alexithymic’ characteristics in psychosomatic patients”, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Vol. 22 Nos 2/6, pp. 255-262.

Sifneos, P.E. (1991), “Affect, emotional conflict, and deficit: an overview”, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Vol. 56 No. 3, pp. 116-122.

Skarlicki, D.P., Folger, R. and Tesluk, P. (1999), “Personality as a moderator in the relationship between fairness and retaliation”, Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 42 No. 1, pp. 100-108.

Škerlavaj, M., Connelly, C.E., Cerne, M. and Dysvik, A. (2018), “Tell me if you can: time pressure, prosocial motivation, perspective taking, and knowledge hiding”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 22 No. 7, pp. 1489-1509.

Taylor, G.J., Ryan, D. and Bagby, M. (1985), “Toward the development of a new self-report alexithymia scale”, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Vol. 44 No. 4, pp. 191-199.

Tett, R.P. and Burnett, D.D. (2003), “A personality trait-based interactionist model of job performance”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 88 No. 3, pp. 500-517.

Thompson, E.R. (2007), “Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS)”, Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Vol. 38 No. 2, pp. 227-242.

Thoresen, C., Kaplan, S., Barsky, A., Warren, C. and Chermont, K. (2003), “The affective underpinnings of job perceptions and attitudes: a meta-analytic review and integration”, Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 129 No. 6, pp. 914-945.

Tolmunen, T., Heliste, M., Lehto, S.M., Hintikka, J., Honkalampi, K. and Kauhanen, J. (2011), “Stability of alexithymia in the general population: an 11-year follow-up”, Comprehensive Psychiatry, Vol. 52 No. 5, pp. 536-541.

van den Hooff, B., Schouten, A.P. and Simonovski, S. (2012), “What one feels and what one knows: the influence of emotions on attitudes and intentions towards knowledge sharing”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 16 No. 1, pp. 148-158.

Vanheule, S., Desmet, M., Meganck, R. and Bogaerts, S. (2007), “Alexithymia and interpersonal problems”, Journal of Clinical Psychology, Vol. 63 No. 1, pp. 109-117.

Watson, D. and Clark, L.A. (1984), “Negative affectivity: the disposition to experience aversive emotional states”, Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 96 No. 3, pp. 465-490.

Watson, D., Clark, L.A. and Tellegen, A. (1988), “Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 54 No. 6, pp. 1063-1070.

Wheeler, L. (1966), “Toward a theory of behavioral contagion”, Psychological Review, Vol. 73 No. 2, pp. 179-192.

Wróbel, M., Finogenow, M., Szymańska, P. and Laurent, J. (2019), “Measuring positive and negative affect in a school-based sample: a polish version of the PANAS-C”, Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, Vol. 41 No. 4, pp. 598-611.

Wu, D. (2020), “Withholding effort in sharing knowledge in online space: differential effects of task characteristics”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 24 No. 10, pp. 2401-2429.

Xiong, C., Chang, V., Scuotto, V., Shi, Y. and Paoloni, N. (2021), “The social-psychological approach in understanding knowledge hiding within international R&D teams: an inductive analysis”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 128, pp. 799-811.

Yang, J. and Diefendorff, J.M. (2009), “The relations of daily counterproductive workplace behavior with emotions, situational antecedents, and personality moderators: a diary study in Hong Kong”, Personnel Psychology, Vol. 62 No. 2, pp. 259-295.

Yao, L.J., Kam, T.H.Y. and Chan, S.H. (2007), “Knowledge sharing in asian public administration sector: the case of Hong Kong”, Journal of Enterprise Information Management, Vol. 20 No. 1, pp. 51-69.

Yao, Z., Zhang, X., Luo, J. and Huang, H. (2020), “Offense is the best defense: the impact of workplace bullying on knowledge hiding”, Journal of Knowledge Management, Vol. 24 No. 3, pp. 675-695.

Zhou, J., Li, X., Tian, L. and Huebner, E.S. (2020), “Longitudinal association between low self-esteem and depression in early adolescents: the role of rejection sensitivity and loneliness”, Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, Vol. 93 No. 1, pp. 54-71.

Corresponding author

Roman Kmieciak can be contacted at: roman.kmieciak@polsl.pl

About the author

Roman Kmieciak is based at Centre for New Technologies, Silesian University of Technology, Gliwice, Poland.

Related articles