Revealing driver psychophysiological response to emergency braking in distracted driving based on field experiments

Ying Li (School of Information Engineering, Chang’an University, Xi’an, China)
Li Zhao (Nebraska Transportation Center, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska, USA)
Kun Gao (Department of Architecture and Civil Engineering, Chalmers University of Technology, Gothenburg, Sweden)
Yisheng An (School of Information Engineering, Chang'an University, Xi’an, China)
Jelena Andric (Volvo Group Trucks Technology, Gothenburg, Sweden)

Journal of Intelligent and Connected Vehicles

ISSN: 2399-9802

Article publication date: 8 August 2022

Issue publication date: 11 October 2022

691

Abstract

Purpose

The purpose of this paper is to characterize distracted driving by quantifying the response time and response intensity to an emergency stop using the driver’s physiological states.

Design/methodology/approach

Field tests with 17 participants were conducted in the connected and automated vehicle test field. All participants were required to prioritize their primary driving tasks while a secondary nondriving task was asked to be executed. Demographic data, vehicle trajectory data and various physiological data were recorded through a biosignalsplux signal data acquisition toolkit, such as electrocardiograph for heart rate, electromyography for muscle strength, electrodermal activity for skin conductance and force-sensing resistor for braking pressure.

Findings

This study quantified the psychophysiological responses of the driver who returns to the primary driving task from the secondary nondriving task when an emergency occurs. The results provided a prototype analysis of the time required for making a decision in the context of advanced driver assistance systems or for rebuilding the situational awareness in future automated vehicles when a driver’s take-over maneuver is needed.

Originality/value

The hypothesis is that the secondary task will result in a higher mental workload and a prolonged reaction time. Therefore, the driver states in distracted driving are significantly different than in regular driving, the physiological signal improves measuring the brake response time and distraction levels and brake intensity can be expressed as functions of driver demographics. To the best of the authors’ knowledge, this is the first study using psychophysiological measures to quantify a driver’s response to an emergency stop during distracted driving.

Keywords

Citation

Li, Y., Zhao, L., Gao, K., An, Y. and Andric, J. (2022), "Revealing driver psychophysiological response to emergency braking in distracted driving based on field experiments", Journal of Intelligent and Connected Vehicles, Vol. 5 No. 3, pp. 270-282. https://doi.org/10.1108/JICV-06-2022-0024

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2022, Ying Li, Li Zhao, Kun Gao, Yisheng An and Jelena Andric.

License

Published in Journal of Intelligent and Connected Vehicles. Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence maybe seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


1. Introduction

According to the National Bureau of Statistics of China (NBSC), approximately 61,703 Chinese were killed in automobile accidents in 2020, and more than 90% of those accidents were caused by human errors (NBSC, National Bureau of Statistics of China, 2021). To this end, advanced driver assistance systems (ADAS) are developed to prevent many human errors causing accidents during the driving process for safety and better driving (Mishra and Kumar, 2021). United States National Highway Traffic Safety Administration reported that distracted driving caused 3,142 lives lost in motor vehicle crashes in 2020 nationwide in the USA (NHTSA, the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 2019). Distracted driving refers to any activities that could divert a motorist’s attention away from the primary task of safe driving. It has been widely acknowledged as a risky behavior that poses a danger to the vehicle occupants and the traffic community (e.g. motorists, pedestrians and bicyclists). Distractions in driving can be generally categorized into three types:

  1. visual, e.g. looking at something other than the road;

  2. manual, e.g. manipulating something other than the steering wheel; and

  3. cognitive, e.g. thinking about something other than driving.

In the actual driving context, the distraction sources are usually complex and may include a combination of one or more of the three types. The most alarming facts are that the driver’s visual, manual and cognitive attention are often altogether distracted away from driving, such as texting or playing mobile games (AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety, 2020). Therefore, it has been attracting a large body of research attention to underline how drivers react when driving with multiple distractions (Qi et al., 2020).

Several studies have reported that distractions cause brake response, especially when braking is urgently needed (Lee et al., 2001; Strayer and Johnston, 2001; NSC, the National Safety Council, 2012; Gao and Davis, 2017). Brake response time is one of the critical surrogate measures for crash risks (Green, 2000; Jurecki and Stanczyk, 2014). The brake response time can be typically obtained by observing the brake lights (Johansson and Rumar, 1971) or measuring the pedal effort (Fitch et al., 2010; Gao and Davis, 2017). In the literature, braking response time varies due to driver’s expectations (Green, 2000), level of attention (Lee et al., 2001), situation urgency (Summala, 2000), as well as differences in measuring methods (Jurecki and Stanczyk, 2011). However, these previous studies designed driving tasks and traffic scenarios without sufficient concern for the driver’s psychophysiological response to emergency braking.

Conventional ADAS use automated technology to detect nearby obstacles or driver errors, which relies on inputs from multiple data sources, including automotive imaging, lidar, radar, image processing and computer vision. Human cognition is a critical factor for traffic accidents and poor driving performance, assessment of covert cognitive states of drivers through psychophysiological measurements is adopted by ADAS to predict and augment risky driving behavior (Lohani et al., 2019). With the rapid growth of sensor technology, researchers began to investigate the behavior of distracted driving with psychophysiological signals such as heart rate, breathing, blood pressure and skin conductance (Grassmann et al., 2016; Haufe et al., 2014). Currently, it has been widely used for investigating drivers’ fatigue and drowsiness (Li et al., 2011), distraction and inattention, mood (Castegnetti et al., 2017) and aggressiveness (Malta et al., 2001). Unsurprisingly, it also shows an innovative way to measure drivers’ brake response time during distractions (Arakawa et al., 2019; Gao et al., 2020).

Past studies associated with measuring the impact of distracted driving on safety can be categorized into three types, including data analysis (Peng and Boyle, 2012; Qin et al., 2019; Jamil et al., 2021), driving simulator studies (Nowosielski et al., 2018; Xu and Lin, 2018; Arkonac et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2019; Baldo et al., 2020) and field studies (Dingus et al., 2016; Ma et al., 2018; Arvin et al., 2019; Hernández-Rojas et al., 2019; Arvin and Khattak, 2020; Gao et al., 2021). Due to driving simulation providing a low-cost method for driver behavior data collection, most distracted driving studies were conducted in a simulator environment. Field study is undertaken within a natural environment, allowing researchers to gain firsthand experience and knowledge about the people, events and processes they study. However, the acquisition of field data is costly and time-consuming. Compared with data analysis and driving simulator study, the field study is a more appropriate approach to quantifying distracted driving behaviors and their influences on other factors (Wu and Xu, 2018; Wijayaratna et al., 2019). Currently, there are few reports of studies related to secondary in-car tasks. Most field studies investigate the naturalistic driving data collected from the cameras and sensors to find the riskiest factors outside the vehicles faced by drivers during driving (Dingus et al., 2016; Arvin and Khattak, 2020). There is still a lack of field tests of drivers’ physiological states of driver distraction involves a secondary task when an emergency brake occurs.

Drivers with experience in using ADAS on more vehicle control tasks, like adaptive cruise control and lane-keeping assist, may increase drivers’ engagement in distracted driving (Arkonac et al., 2019; Hungund et al., 2021; Xue et al., 2021). With the developments in the field of wearable physiological sensors (Lu et al., 2020), the additional information collected from the driver will be used to improve the ADAS. This study attempted to outline an explanatory framework for emergency brakes in distracted driving situations, characterized by driver demographics, vehicle kinematics and more importantly physiological signals. Using a biosignalsplux signal data acquisition toolkit, an experiment was properly designed to characterize distracted driving related to secondary in-car tasks by quantifying the response time and response intensity to an emergency stop using the driver’s physiological states. Specifically, the paper attempts to answer the question when the driver of a vehicle is suddenly faced with a critical risk of collision, how fast and strong an evasive maneuver (e.g. brake) is applied given the driver is distracted driving. The proposed framework will guide the potential causal role of distraction load when drivers use ADAS.

2. Experiment setup and testing field

The test field is located at the automobile test ring road of Chang’an University in Xi’an, China. This oval-shaped, closed test ring road measures 2.4 km long in total, complemented by a 1.1 km straight road on its central axis. The experiments were conducted at the northeast corner of the test field where parts of the ring road and the straight road form a loop, as indicated in Figure 1. The test vehicle is a compact SUV manufactured by BYD auto group in China. The vehicle model is BYD Yuan EV360 with front-wheel drive and automatic transmission. It should be noted that the test vehicle is electric-powered, and electric vehicles’ driver behavior patterns will be different from those of traditional gasoline vehicles. There are some differences in driving characteristics between electric vehicles and gasoline vehicles, particularly a stronger accelerating and decelerating within the first experiences with electric vehicles (Helmbrecht et al., 2014).

During the experiments, the focus is on using advanced measurement devices to collect data about the driver’s physiological states. Biosignalsplux, a body sensor system from PluxTM, Portugal, is adopted in this study. It provides solutions to acquire reliable psychophysiological signal data by integrating various user-selectable sensors, working with Bluetooth and internal memory. Because of its low-cost, purpose-built and self-wearable, the toolkit is ideal for field driving tests. Figure 2 shows the Biosignalsplux signal data acquisition toolkit used in this study. An eight-channel wearable hub is plugged into eight amplifiers (i.e. body sensors), and their succinct applications are listed below:

  1. Channel 1: Electrocardiography (ECG) for heart activity;

  2. Channel 2: Electromyography (EMG) for muscle activity (e.g. the right calf);

  3. Channel 3: Force-sensing resistor (FSR) for braking force;

  4. Channel 4: Electrodermal activity (EDA) for skin conductance;

  5. Channel 5: Piezoelectric respiration (PZT) for respiration change;

  6. Channel 6: Temperature (TMP) for body surface temperature;

  7. Channel 7: Accelerometer (ACC) for vehicle braking deceleration rate; and

  8. Channel 8: Electroencephalography (EEG) for brain activity.

Body sensors (i.e. ECG, EMG, EDA and EEG) used pregelled (coated with Ag/AgCl polymer and conductive/adhesive hydrogel), disposable, snap-on electrode patches. As shown in Figure 2, these peel-and-stick disposable electrodes are buckled onto the sensor heads and attached to the corresponding skin surface of the human body. In addition to the eight-channel sensors, the system also includes a marker (see Figure 2), which is a handheld switch for event annotation. In the time-stamped data recording, the period of a certain event can be marked by continuously pressing or releasing the button (pressing = 1, releasing = 0). It facilitates locating the event in later data review or processing without affecting the functionality of the system. The system comes with software that is a suite of data analysis add-ons for real-time signal visualization and recording. Data are acquired from all channels simultaneously and transmitted to the local server (e.g. laptop) wirelessly.

The experiments were conducted from 19 to 26 August 2019. Because the test field was closed (nonpublic), no pedestrians or other vehicles would be present on the test track. At the beginning of the experiment, all the drivers wore the toolkit of the body sensors by professional staff to ensure the correct setup. Afterward, the divers were required to drive the vehicle on the test loop and execute different driving tasks. During driving, the participants were asked to perform three hard braking maneuvers in response to a clear verbal “stop” order given by the experiment assistant in the car. The “stop” order was randomly given when vehicles were running on a straight section of the test site. The time gap between any two “stop” orders was greater than 1 minute to avoid confound effects.

In the worst-case scenario where drivers are distracted in all three ways – visual, manual and cognitive, a secondary task of playing a game on a mobile phone was designed. That said, two driving scenarios were tested for each participant:

  1. regular driving when safe driving is the primary and only task of the participant; and

  2. distracted driving where drivers are asked to execute a secondary nondriving task (mobile game) under the premise of safe driving.

Figure 3 shows the status of the participants in the two driving scenarios during the test. The source of driving distraction is designed to play a coloring game. In the game, a simple picture of seven colors is pixelated into 618 equal cells. Participants can color it by numbers (1 through 7 correspond to seven given colors) marked in the cell. Participants were told that the final game score consisted of gaining points for correct coloring and double losing points for incorrect coloring, and a higher score would be rewarded. The participants must pay enough attention to their mobile phones to handle the game properly. In contrast to regular driving, participants’ visual, manual and cognitive abilities would be affected to varying degrees by distracted driving. Specifically, the participant’s vision must switch back and forth between the road and the mobile screen; the participant holds the mobile phone with one hand which is off the steering wheel; and the participant allocates some cognition to the mobile game.

3. Experiment process and data collection

3. 1 Experiment process

According to the experiment design in this study, the data acquisition procedure mainly includes five steps, as illustrated in Figure 4. Each step is elaborated on below.

First, the introductions about the experiments were provided to participants including showing the test site and getting familiar with the test vehicle. Participants were told that the upper-speed limit of the test site was 40 km/h, but they can drive at their self-evaluated safe speed. For example, they may slow down when they are in uncertain driving conditions, limited sight distance or distracted. Besides the driving task, all participants were told that an emergency stop order would be randomly released by the assistant sitting next to them, and that they should act immediately once heard. The experiment lasts about 30 minutes for each participant.

Next, the experimental staff attached the electrodes of four sensors onto the corresponding locations of the participant’s bodies. More specifically, the three-lead ECG (for heart activity) was attached to the participant’s left chests. EMG for muscle activity during braking was measured with three electrodes, two positioned at the right calf muscles and one stuck to the right ankle (less muscle) as the benchmark. EDA for skin conductance attached two electrodes to the sole of the foot (mostly left foot). EEG (for brain activity) had three electrodes: one long electrode was stuck onto the forehead while the other short electrodes were on the back of the head. The technical details about obtaining physiological signal data from the equipment are available in PLUX (2019) and Braithwaite et al. (2013). The PZT sensor for respiration was tied to the participant’s chest by an elastic strap. A localized sensing element was embedded to measure displacement variations due to inhaling or exhaling. Moreover, TMP was used for skin temperature with only one contact thermistor taped to the skin surface of the leg. Another two sensors are not on the driver. The FSR sensor was taped onto the center of the braking pedal to measure the braking force. The ACC sensor was firmly taped on the vehicle to measure the deceleration on the longitudinal of the vehicle.

In regular and distracted driving situations, participants were asked to take an emergency stop. Specifically, participants randomly received a verbal “stop” order from the experiment assistant in the vehicle. After receiving the order, the participants must brake to a complete stop. The participants were told that this “stop” order was to mimic emergency braking scenarios such as a sudden jaywalker cutting in front or almost hitting another car. The urgent and compact “stop” order forced drivers to brake to the best of their ability as soon as possible. A break of approximately 5 minutes was designed between the tests of the two driving situations to eliminate successive effects.

3.2 Participants

As listed in Table 1, 17 participants between the age of 20 and 59 who hold a license ranging from less than 1 year to 20 years were recruited. The participants come from different occupational backgrounds and are overall in good health status (e.g. free from drowsiness, illness and drunkenness). Prior to the experiment, verbal consent for voluntary participation was received from all participants. The basic information of the participants including age, gender, driving experience and education level (e.g. high school degree, university degree and graduate degree) was collected as well. After the experiment, each participant was compensated 50 RMB for their participations. The 17 participants include 3 females and 14 males, with the educational attainments ranging from middle school to PhD degree. The average age was 33.2 years with a standard deviation (SD) of 10.8 and the average license-hold was 7.8 years with a SD of 7.1. The mean driving experience is 6.4 years with a SD of 7.6. To be more generally representative, the experiment attempted to balance the demographics of the participant samples as much as possible.

3.3 Data collection

The collected data during the experiments mainly include the eight-channel physiological signals (i.e. ECG, EMG, FSR, EDA, PZT, TEP, ACC and EEG). The characteristic parameters were mainly focused on time-domain features (e.g. the response of the signal) and intensity-domain features (e.g. the amplitude of the signal). Note that the only difference of data recording between the two driving situations (regular vs distracted driving) is the need to save the game results for the distracted driving. The data acquisition system is set with a resolution of 16 bits and a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz for all channels. In addition, the vehicle trajectory, including latitude, longitude, instantaneous speed and time stamps, were also recorded every 0.1 seconds using a GPS mobile application software. Physiological signal and vehicle trajectory data were synchronized in time series.

4. Data process and analysis methods

4.1 Vehicular speed profiles

The speed profile data (SPD) before and after the “stop” order (i.e. stimuli) were extracted from the originally recorded vehicle trajectory data set. The timestamp when the stop order is issued is set to zero (i.e. benchmark), and the time prior to the stop event is labeled as negative. As shown in Figure 5, the trajectory data of all stop events are normalized, in time, so that all speed profiles are in a time window of negative 4–7 seconds.

Three parameters were used to characterize the SPD:

  1. initial speed, obtained by averaging the speeds prior to the event (i.e. t < 0 in Figure 5);

  2. deceleration rate, obtained by calculating the slope of the linear part of the speed reduction; and

  3. reaction time, obtained by measuring the duration from the start of the event (t = 0) to the time when the speed reduced to 10% of the initial speed.

SPD of the first three participants is not included as partial data of the three participants are missing. For the sake of robustness, the full data from 14 participants are analyzed.

4.2 Physiological signal data

Figure 6 provides examples of different physiological signal data in the regular driving (in grey) and distracted driving (in red) situations. By manually checking the pattern of the data, TMP and EEG signal data were not distinctive or informative parameters. Therefore, these two signals were not interpreted in the following study.

As shown in Figure 6, each profile represents a signal data change over time prior to and post to a stop event. Each participant received at least three “stop” orders in each driving situation. The average performance of the “stop” even was used for analysis to reduce the random error. Figure 7 summarizes the characteristics of each physiological signal. The parameters to characterize different physiological signals are related to the amplitude and time of critical changes. The unit of amplitude corresponds to each physiological signal and the units of all time-related parameters (e.g. timestamp, duration and interval) are in seconds. These parameters can be either extracted directly from the signal data, as illustrated in the last column of Figure 7, or calculated from the parameters obtained. A preliminary data cleaning process was conducted to delete some outliers or incomplete data. For example, the electrode of a specific sensor may be off from the participant accidentally during the test, resulting in no change or illegible change in the obtained signal data. Those signal data were excluded from the analysis result presented in Section 4.3.

4.3 Second-task performances

The participants’ distraction level is quantified by their completion degree of the designed mobile game. In other words, it is hypothesized that the better the game is completed, the higher the mental load is required, and thus the higher the distraction level is reached. Conversely, it is hypothesized that a worse game completion refers to a lower distraction level, as participants assign more attention to the primary driving task rather than the secondary nondriving task (i.e. mobile game). Examples of game snapshots in low, medium and high distraction levels are shown in Figure 8. The game score is calculated from the “gain-points” and “lose-points” for each participant. With a total of 618 digital pixel cells in each snapshot in Figure 8, the gain point is counted when a cell is correctly colored, while incorrect coloring results in double lose points. The scoring rate (SR) is calculated as the quotient of the total scores and the game duration is adopted to measure the game scoring performance, as shown in Table 2.

In Table 2, the distraction SR of 16 participants is investigated considering different durations of the finishing game (due to the lack of data, participant Driver ID = 3 was not included). The scoring rate is divided into three levels to reflect the distraction load of a driver: low (SR ≤ 0.5), medium (0.5 ≤ SR < 1) and high (SR > 1).

5. Results and discussions

5.1 Speed profile analysis result

The results of the speed profile data analysis are summarized in Table 3. It was found that, on average, the initial speed of distracted driving (31 km/h) is lower than the initial speed of regular driving (36.5 km/h), and the difference is significant (p = 0.002). This was not surprising, in the literature, distracted drivers have been repeatedly reported to compensate for the risk of distraction by reducing their driving speeds (Caird et al., 2008). The difference in average deceleration rate for the two driving situations was also significant (p = 0.004), noting that a harder brake was taken when an emergency “stop” was required in regular driving (avg. deceleration = −2.96 m/s2) as compared to the distracted driving (avg. deceleration = −2.09 m/s2). This may indicate the distracted drivers did not fully brake. The study hypothesized that distracted drivers take a longer time on average to respond to the “stop” order. However, it was not confirmed statistically in the data. It should be noted that due to the inherent accuracy of this method, the reaction time obtained by the SPD may not necessarily reflect the actual difference. The following physiological data may provide a better solution to this problem.

5.2 Physiological factor analysis and results

Table 4 provides the statistics of the physiological signal data(i.e. FSR, ACC, EMG, PZT, ECG, ECG and EDA), including the average values and the standard deviations in the parentheses. A paired two-tailed t-test with a p-value is performed to test if there are significant differences in the investigated indicators between distracted and regular driving. The p-value is marked in bold red when the test is rejected at the confidence level of 0.05, and in bold black when the p-value is marginally significant (at the confidence level of 0.1).

5.2.1 Kinematic signals – accelerometer and force-sensing resistor

In Table 4 (1), ACC data find two significant parameters, namely the maximum amplitude of the deceleration and the rise time in distracted driving. These two parameters correspond to the significance of the average deceleration rate. As for the response time, there was no significant evidence found in ACC signal data. In Table 4 (2), the intensity of the brake force, extracted from the FSR signal, is prominent with an average maximum of 0.297 and 0.262 kg for distracted driving and regular driving, respectively. Three key periods: response time, rise time and recovery time, were measured and the rise time was found significant in distracted driving as compared to regular driving (p = 0.006). Although the amplitude of the FSR may be remarkable for the intensity of the response, it is not significant (p = 0.542). This is because the sensor head attached to the brake pedal is tiny and thin, so it is easy to miss the partial braking force of the foot on the pedal. It is found in the FSR data that the amplitudes differ greatly from each other. To interpret the kinematics of the vehicle, distracted drivers tend to respond slower, pressed the brakes harder and had shorter times to maximum brake pedal depression than regular drivers. This may partially be because distracted drivers who immediately return to execute the stop maneuver, are not quite sure about the external condition and thus drivers are not able to sharply brake.

5.2.2 Physiological signals – electromyography and electrodermal activity

Evidence can also be found from the EMG signal data in Table 4 (3) to indicate the significance of the rise time in distracted driving (p = 0.027). EMG finds marginal evidence for the significance of the response time (p = 0.057). Therefore, detecting the physiological signal of the calf muscles during braking is more powerful than catching the force applied on the brake pedal (i.e. FSR) to distinguish between distracted driving and regular driving. It can be found in Table 4 (4) that, on average, the initial EDA level of distracted drivers is significantly higher than that of regular drivers (p-value = 0.005). When the event of an emergency stop is activated, skin conductance responds to it clearly, a drastic increase of the max amplitude of the skin conductance (i.e. the third column vs the second column) is observed. There may be marginal evidence for the difference in skin conductance response time (p = 0.070) and rise time (p = 0.067), indicating that the distracted driver may take longer time to react to the emergency stop than the regular drivers. The current study found that drivers’ mean response time to an expected stop event was 0.55 seconds. These response times obtained from the physiological signals are far less than the reported brake response time, which is 1.5 seconds (Green, 2000) as generally recommended in the literature. This is not surprising as physiological signals are always the first (among other measures) to react after receiving an external stimulus, and it responds even faster at higher urgencies (Markkula et al., 2016). The rise time reflects the speed at which the physiological signal reaches its maximum amplitude, i.e. the response intensity. The results show that distracted driving needs less time to reach its maximum compared to regular driving. This is interesting because distracted drivers can be described as “delayed but hurry and intensive responders.”

5.2.3 Periodic signals – piezoelectric respiration and electrocardiography.

For periodic signals such as PZT and ECG in Table 4 (5) and (6), the time intervals between consecutive two peaks were measured (i.e. breath-to-breath and beat-to-beat), as shown in equation (1). The reciprocal of the average peak-peak intervals, i.e. the frequency, is the breath rate or heart rate, as calculated by equation (2). The breath rate variability or heart rate variability can be indicated using the root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) of the peak-peak intervals, as can be found in equation (3).

(1) PPIi,tPZT|ECG=Pr+1,tPZT|ECG-Pr,tPZT|ECG
(2) Freqtbr|hr=60*Nti=1NtPPIiPZT|ECG
(3) RMSSDtbrv|hrv=1Nt-1(i=1Nt-1(PPIi+1PZT|ECG-PPIiPZT|ECG)2)

where:

PPI= peak-peak intervals measured from PZT or ECG signal;

Freq= frequency of breath rate (br, unit: breath/min) or heart rate (hr, unit: beat/min) depending on what physiological signal is analyzed;

RMSSD = root mean square of successive differences for breath rate variability (brv, unit: sec) or heart rate variability (hrv, unit: sec) depending on what physiological signal is analyzed;

Nt= number of the total intervals in the predefined study period t;

i= interval index, i = 1, 2, …, N − 1;

r= peak index, r = 1, 2, …, N + 1; and

t= the predefined study time window.

Some cognitive states may be inferred from respiration amplitude responses after external events (Castegnetti, 2017). In the PZT signal data, the respiration amplitude is significant prior to the event (p = 0.011) and is marginal significant post the “stop” event (p = 0.085). The heart rate variability indicates mental stress and anxiety corresponding to physiological changes in the autonomic nervous systems. When the emergency stop is required, both heart rate (i.e. RR interval) and heart rate variability (i.e. RMSSD) are significant in distracted driving. Figure 9 shows the prior-post comparison test within one driving situation. Dashed lines with p-values are used to indicate the t-test result. Each bar is marked with the mean in the center and the 95% confidence intervals in the lower and upper boundaries.

Figure 9(a) shows that heart rate has no difference whether drivers are distracted or not (p = 0.495) prior to the activation of the stop event. However, the heart rate of distracted drivers increases marginally significantly (p = 0.096) than that of regular driving after the stop event. These marginal effects were further demonstrated in Figure 9(b) to be significant in heart rate variability (p = 0.006). This is because heart rate variability can identify minor differences, and thus is more informative (Sacha, 2014). Results show that the heart rate variabilities in distracted driving and regular driving increased after the event (p = 0.006 and p = 0.059). The difference in the increase is significantly different as distracted driving gains more variability (p = 0.009).

Both novice drivers (ID = 13, 17) and experienced drivers (ID = 7, 9) have a low level of distraction (i.e. game score), implying that comparatively less attention is assigned to the secondary task. This may be due to these reasons:

  • novice drivers are inexperienced in driving and have no extra ability to take the additional nondriving tasks;

  • experienced drivers are conservative in safety driving; and

  • those experienced drivers are aged 40–60 years, and are relatively unskilled about smartphone games compared to the generation (aged 20–40 years) that grew up with smartphones.

Driver’s age and experience were reported to affect the emergency braking (Loeb et al., 2015), especially on the brake response time and intensity. As can be found in Figure 10(a), the results in this study corroborated the findings in previous studies (Loeb et al., 2015). Figure 10(b) shows that the driver’s rise time decreases with the increase of either the age or driving experience. The results indicated that a less intense response is conducted by elder drivers as well as experienced drivers.

6. Conclusion and discussions

This paper outlines an explanatory framework for emergency brakes in distracted driving situations, characterized by driver demographics, vehicle kinematics and more importantly, physiological signals. This study extends the literature findings on the psychophysiological measures of distracted driving when an emergency stop is required. In automated driving mode, drivers do not need to monitor the environment all the time and can engage in their secondary tasks. When the autonomous system detects an emergency situation that requires the driver to take over, the brake features in this study work as a preliminary study. As take-over quality deteriorated for distracted drivers (Zeeb et al., 2016), this study underlined how quick drivers could respond to the “take over” notice (released by the autonomous driving system) and resume vehicle control. Eventually, the proposed framework may serve to guide the next step of this study on the potential causal role of distraction load when drivers are in autonomous vehicles.

Recently, various forms of wearable physiological monitoring devices are commercially available. Therefore, it is possible to get the physiological signals of drivers to improve the ADAS. The driver assistance system would moderate the response time when distracted (Zeeb et al., 2016), and it would provide a reference for monitoring the physiological state (Hamdani and Fernando, 2015; Vetter et al., 2017). The driver state could provide information to a system that decides the way to adapt the vehicle based on the demands of the task relative to the distraction state (NHTSA, the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 2004). The particularity of this experiment is the simplification of the driver’s response to the emergency stop event. In most of the previous distraction experiments, the driver first perceives an event, decides and produces a response, i.e. the classical perceive–decide–response mode. In this study, the driver was informed of the emergency brake and performed it immediately after a quick external condition check. In this study, however, the experiment assistant acted as the perceiver and the decision-maker of the event. The driver’s physiological states can be monitored so to leverage technology to improve road safety, such as to assist in driver distraction detection and warning system for crash avoidance. This study demonstrates that the drivers are physiologically reactive to urgent stimuli, especially drivers under distraction. It provides a viable means of detecting and distinguishing distracted driving status. The main findings are summarized as below.

Before the emergency stop event, distracted drivers run with a relatively low speed (SPD, p = 0.002) and are generally more nervous (EDA, p = 0.005; PZT, p = 0.011). When responding to the stop request, distracted drivers are delayed in perception (EMG, p = 0.057; EDA, p = 0.070), and hurriedly reached to the maximum intensity response indicated by the maximum amplitude (PZT, p = 0.085; ECG, p = 0.041) and the rise time (ACC, 0.021; FSR, p = 0.006; EMG, p = 0.027; EDA, p = 0.067) and do not get the full brake (SPD, p = 0.004; ACC, p = 0.002). These lead to an escalated stress and anxiety (PZT, p = 0.013; ECG, p = 0.009). All these results help to characterize and differentiate the distracted drivers.

When the response time collection method moves from vehicle kinematics (i.e. SPD, FSR, ACC) to physiological signal (i.e. EMG, EDA), more clear evidence can be found to distinguish a distracted driving state. The order of the response time: EMG < FSR < ACC < SPD makes good sense as the driver firstly lifted his or her right leg (EMG), pressed the brake pedal (FSR), the vehicle decelerated (ACC) and then captured by the trajectory device (SPD). The distracted drivers are physiologically more reactive to the emergency stop stimuli, exhibiting significant increases in the intensity and time of the response. Among all the physiological parameters tested in this study, response time and rise time are sensitive and recommended in distracted driving studies. Although amplitude is fundamental in reflecting the reaction intensity, it should be cautious as it may require a more accurate data acquisition system with multiple channels for each signal.

One of the limitations of the study is the small sample size. The results, therefore, should be interpreted with caution and wait for repetition to arrive at a firm conclusion. A repeated measure of multilevel modeling that adopts the driver-nested data structure is suggested in the future study when the data sample is enlarged to capture the within-subject variability. The design of the experiment could be further improved as well. For example, the order of the two driving tasks carried out by the participants may be reversed to help eliminate biases caused by the previous driving experience. What’s more, the accuracy of the measurements was dependent on the reliability of the assistant’s action time. Although the body sensor system was sensitive to many signals in a preexperimental test (e.g. deep breathing, muscle contracting and sudden frightening), its accuracy has not been validated, especially given driving is a complicated process that many factors may be out of control.

Figures

The test site at Chang’an University

Figure 1

The test site at Chang’an University

Biosignalsplux data acquisition system

Figure 2

Biosignalsplux data acquisition system

Regular driving (left) and distracted driving (right) in the test

Figure 3

Regular driving (left) and distracted driving (right) in the test

Data acquisition procedure

Figure 4

Data acquisition procedure

Examples of vehicle speed profiles for each participant

Figure 5

Examples of vehicle speed profiles for each participant

Physiological signal data in distracted and regular driving situations (Driver ID = 13)

Figure 6

Physiological signal data in distracted and regular driving situations (Driver ID = 13)

Characteristic parameters of each physiological signal

Figure 7

Characteristic parameters of each physiological signal

Snapshots of driver’s digital coloring task indicating high (left), medium (middle) and low (right) for Driver ID = 1, 8 and 11, respectively

Figure 8

Snapshots of driver’s digital coloring task indicating high (left), medium (middle) and low (right) for Driver ID = 1, 8 and 11, respectively

Heart rate comparison (a) and heart rate variability comparison (b) between distracted driving (i.e. dis) and regular driving (i.e. reg) prior to and post to the event, respectively

Figure 9

Heart rate comparison (a) and heart rate variability comparison (b) between distracted driving (i.e. dis) and regular driving (i.e. reg) prior to and post to the event, respectively

Driver’s age and experience (Driver IDs are labeled next to the data points)

Figure 10

Driver’s age and experience (Driver IDs are labeled next to the data points)

Driver demographics summary

Driver ID Gender Educational attainment Age License-hold year Estimated actual driving year
1 Female > bachelor’s degree 33 9 2
2 Female > bachelor’s degree 32 5 5
3 Female > bachelor’s degree 37 0.5 0.1
4 Male ≤ bachelor’s degree 40 7 5
5 Male ≤ bachelor’s degree 37 4 4
6 Male > bachelor’s degree 51 18 18
7 Male ≤ bachelor’s degree 59 22 22
8 Male > bachelor’s degree 24 6 1
9 Male > bachelor’s degree 45 20 20
10 Male ≤ bachelor’s degree 36 17 17
11 Male ≤ bachelor’s degree 24 5 5
12 Male > bachelor’s degree 25 4 1
13 Male > bachelor’s degree 25 0.1 0.1
14 Male > bachelor’s degree 24 4 2
15 Male > bachelor’s degree 29 7 4
16 Male > bachelor’s degree 24 3 3
17 Male ≤ bachelor’s degree 20 0.3 0.1

Distraction load summary

Driver ID Duration (s) Gain (pts) Lose (pts) Total scoring rate SR (pts/s) Distraction load level
1 272 341 8 1.19 high
2 310 361 8 1.11 high
4 292 159 7 0.49 low
5 274 324 3 1.16 high
6 196 249 10 1.17 high
7 326 152 21 0.34 low
8 187 141 4 0.71 medium
9 176 239 1 1.35 high
10 354 269 12 0.69 medium
11 260 60 19 0.08 low
12 219 177 1 0.80 medium
13 338 247 8 0.68 medium
14 250 273 13 0.99 medium
15 261 159 10 0.53 medium
16 202 254 12 1.14 high
17 365 154 11 0.36 low

Characteristic parameters of SPD

Driver ID Initial speed (km/h) Avg. deceleration rate (m/s2) Response time (s)
Distracted Regular Distracted Regular Distracted Regular
4 34.8 40.3 –2.80 –3.24 1.3 2.4
5 36.3 44.6 –1.54 –3.34 2 2.9
6 33.9 32.7 –1.77 –3.26 2.1 1.1
7 29.0 48.0 −0.83 –3.17 1 0.3
8 27.7 34.1 –2.02 –2.07 2.1 1.4
9 36.5 38.5 –3.79 –2.59 1 1.4
10 28.4 29.6 –2.25 –2.83 1.4 1.1
11 32.1 39.8 –2.79 –4.25 1.8 1.5
12 22.0 32.2 –1.71 –3.36 2.2 1.1
13 27.9 30.3 –2.14 –2.24 2.6 2
14 27.8 34.9 –2.19 –2.93 1.4 2.2
15 38.3 40.6 –1.44 –3.23 1.2 3.1
16 36.0 40.1 –1.83 –2.44 2.6 2.7
17 23.9 24.6 –2.22 –2.42 2.6 1.5
Avg. 31.0 36.5 –2.09 –2.96 1.81 1.76
Std. 5.1 6.3 0.71 0.57 0.59 0.81
P–value 0.002* 0.004* 0.866
Note:

*Means significance at the confidence level of 99%

Summary of the physiological signal data

(1) ACC Initial acceleration Max amplitude Response time Rise time Recovery time
N 17 17 17 17 17
Distracted 0.212 (0.087) −0.314 (0.188) 0.725 (0.134) 0.663 (0.322) 1.741 (0.704)
Regular 0.222 (0.075) −0.372 (0.162) 0.679 (0.168) 0.894 (0.369) 1.623 (0.639)
p-value 0.339 0.002 0.115 0.021 0.322
(2) FSR Max amplitude Response time Rise time Recovery time
N 16 16 16
Distracted 0.297(0.164) 0.621 (0.132) 0.322 (0.141) 1.496 (0.987)
Regular 0.262 (0.189) 0.561 (0.134) 0.487 (0.214) 1.546 (0.869)
p-value 0.542 0.112 0.006 0.738
(3) EMG Max amplitude Time at max amplitude Response time Rise time
N 16 16 16 16
Distracted 0.204 (0.136) 0.510 (0.181) 0.399 (0.135) 0.111 (0.068)
Regular 0.205 (0.123) 0.495 (0.241) 0.310 (0.151) 0.186 (0.122)
p-value 0.944 0.823 0.057 0.027
(4) EDA Initial EDA level Max amplitude Response time Rise time 50% Recovery time
N 15 12 12 12 10
Distracted 12.853 (5.133) 15.319 (5.095) 1.815 (0.716) 1.644 (0.736) 1.898 (0.884)
Regular 11.229 (4.989) 14.635 (5.431) 1.513 (0.610) 2.089 (0.736) 1.466 (0.796)
p-value 0.005 0.401 0.070 0.067 0.201
(5) PZT Initial respiration level Max amplitude Root mean square of amplitudes Avg. breath-to-breath interval Avg. RMSSD
N 17 17 17 17 17
Distracted 1.779 (0.270) 1.936 (0.327) 1.785 (0.272) 2.082 (0.481) 2.242 (0.517)
Regular 1.614 (0.233) 1.810 (0.369) 1.624 (0.240) 2.215 (0.408) 2.396 (0.494)
p-value 0.011 0.085 0.013 0.345 0.323
(6) ECG Avg. beat-to-beat interval
prior event
Avg. beat-to-beat interval
post event
Avg. RMSSD
prior event
Avg. RMSSD
post event
N 15 15 15 15
Distracted 0.748 (0.115) 0.726 (0.121) 0.044 (0.047) 0.145 (0.123)
Regular 0.753 (0.103) 0.760 (0.099) 0.048 (0.061) 0.096 (0.112)
p-value 0.646 0.041 0.851 0.009

References

AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety (2020), 2019 Traffic Safety Culture Index (Technical Report) AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety, Washington, DC.

Arakawa, T., Hibi, R. and Fujishiro, T.A. (2019), “Psychophysical assessment of a driver’s mental state in autonomous vehicles”, Transportation Research Part A: policy and Practice, Vol. 124, pp. 587-610.

Arkonac, S.E., Brumby, D.P., Smith, T. and Babu, H.V.R. (2019), “In-car distractions and automated driving: a preliminary simulator study”, In Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications: adjunct Proceedings, pp. 346-351.

Arvin, R., Kamrani, M. and Khattak, A.J. (2019), “The role of pre-crash driving instability in contributing to crash intensity using naturalistic driving data”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 132, pp. 105-226.

Arvin, R. and Khattak, A.J. (2020), “Driving impairments and duration of distractions: assessing crash risk by harnessing microscopic naturalistic driving data”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 146, pp. 105-733, doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2019.07.002.

Baldo, N., Marini, A. and Miani, M. (2020), “Drivers' braking behavior affected by cognitive distractions: an experimental investigation with a virtual car simulator”, Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 10 No. 10, p. 150, doi: 10.3390/bs10100150.

Braithwaite, J.J., Watson, D.G., Jones, R. and Rowe, M. (2013), “A guide for analyzing electrodermal activity (EDA) and skin conductance responses (SCRs) for psychological experiments”, Psychophysiology, Vol. 49 No. 1, pp. 1017-1034.

Caird, J.K., Willness, C.R., Steel, P. and Scialfa, C. (2008), “A meta-analysis of the effects of cell phones on driver performance”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 40 No. 4, pp. 1282-1293.

Castegnetti, G., Tzovara, A., Staib, M., Gerster, S. and Bach, D.R. (2017), “Assessing fear learning via conditioned respiratory amplitude responses”, Psychophysiology, Vol. 54 No. 2, pp. 215-223.

Dingus, T.A., Guo, F., Lee, S., Antin, J.F., Perez, M., Buchanan-King, M. and Hankey, J. (2016), “Driver crash risk factors and prevalence evaluation using naturalistic driving data”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Vol. 113 No. 10, pp. 2636-2641, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1513271113.

Fitch, G.M., Blanco, M., Morgan, J.F. and Wharton, A.E. (2010), “Driver braking performance to surprise and expected events”, Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, Vol. 54 No. 24, pp. 2075-2080.

Gao, J. and Davis, G.A. (2017), “Using naturalistic driving study data to investigate the impact of driver distraction on driver’s brake reaction time in freeway rear-end events in car-following situation”, Journal of Safety Research, Vol. 63, pp. 195-204, doi: 10.1016/j.jsr.2017.10.012.

Gao, K., Tu, H., Sun, L., Sze, N.N., Song, Z. and Shi, H. (2020), “Impacts of reduced visibility under hazy weather condition on collision risk and car-following behavior: implications for traffic control and management”, International Journal of Sustainable Transportation, Vol. 14 No. 8, pp. 635-642.

Gao, K., Yang, Y. and Qu, X. (2021), “Examining nonlinear and interaction effects of multiple determinants on airline travel satisfaction”, Transportation Research Part D: Transport and Environment, Vol. 97, p. 102957.

Grassmann, M., Vlemincx, E., Leupoldt, A., Mittelstädt, J.M. and Van den Bergh, O. (2016), “Respiratory changes in response to cognitive load: a systematic review”, Neural Plasticity, Vol. 2016, pp.1-16.

Green, M. (2000), “How long does it take to stop? Methodological analysis of driver perception-brake times”, Transportation Human Factors, Vol. 2 No. 3, pp. 195-216.

Hamdani, S. and Fernando, A. (2015), “The application of a piezo-resistive cardiorespiratory sensor system in an automobile safety belt”, Sensors, Vol. 15 No. 4, pp. 7742-7753.

Haufe, S., Kim, J.W., Kim, I.H., Sonnleitner, A., Schrauf, M., Curio, G. and Blankertz, B. (2014), “Electrophysiology-based detection of emergency braking intention in real-world driving”, Journal of Neural Engineering, Vol. 11 No. 5.

Helmbrecht, M., Olaverri-Monreal, C., Bengler, K., Vilimek, R. and Keinath, A. (2014), “How electric vehicles affect driving behavioral patterns”, IEEE Intelligent Transportation Systems Magazine, Vol. 6 No. 3, pp. 22-32.

Hernández-Rojas, L.G., Martínez, E. and Antelis, J.M. (2019), “Detection of emergency braking intention using driver's electroencephalographic signals”, IEEE Latin America Transactions, Vol. 17 No. 1, pp. 111-118.

Hungund, A.P., Pai, G. and Pradhan, A.K. (2021), “Systematic review of research on driver distraction in the context of advanced driver assistance systems”, Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board, Vol. 2675 No. 9, pp. 756-765.

Jamil, A., Tabassum, S., Younis, M.W., Khan, A.H., Rehman, Z. and Sanaullah, I. (2021), “Analytical study to find the impacts of using a mobile phone on driver's inattentions while driving–a case study of Lahore”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 157, pp. 106-132, doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2021.106132.

Johansson, G. and Rumar, K. (1971), “Drivers’ brake reaction times”, Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 13 No. 1, pp. 23-27.

Jurecki, R.S. and Stanczyk, T.L. (2011), “The test methods and the reaction time of drivers”, Eksploatacja i Niezawodnosc-Maintenance and Reliability, Vol. 3, pp. 84-91.

Jurecki, R.S. and Stanczyk, T.L. (2014), “Driver reaction time to lateral entering pedestrian in a simulated crash traffic situation”, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, Vol. 27, pp. 22-36.

Lee, J.D., Caven, B., Haake, S. and Brown, T.L. (2001), “Speech-based interaction with in-vehicle computers: the effect of speech-based e-mail on drivers’ attention to the roadway”, Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 43 No. 4, pp. 631-640, doi: 10.1518/001872001775870340.

Li, S., Wang, L., Yang, Z., Ji, B., Qiao, F. and Yang, Z. (2011), “An active driver fatigue identification technique using multiple physiological features”, In Proceedings of 2011 International Conference on Mechatronic Science, IEEE Electric Engineering and Computer, pp. 733-737.

Loeb, H.S., Kandadai, V., McDonald, C.C. and Winston, F.K. (2015), “Emergency braking in adults versus novice teen drivers: response to simulated sudden driving events”, Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board, Vol. 2516 No. 1, pp. 8-14.

Lohani, M., Payne, B.R. and Strayer, D.L. (2019), “A review of psychophysiological measures to assess cognitive states in real-world driving”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, Vol. 13, p. 57.

Lu, L., Zhang, J., Xie, Y., Gao, F., Xu, S., Wu, X. and Ye, Z. (2020), “Wearable health devices in health care: narrative systematic review”, JMIR mHealth and uHealth, Vol. 8 No. 11, p. e18907.

Ma, Y., Hu, B., Chan, C.Y., Qi, S. and Fan, L. (2018), “Distractions intervention strategies for in-vehicle secondary tasks: an on-road test assessment of driving task demand based on real-time traffic environment”, Transportation Research Part D: Transport and Environment, Vol. 63, pp. 747-754, doi: 10.1016/j.trd.2018.07.005.

Malta, L.S., Blanchard, E.B., Freidenberg, B.M., Galovski, T.E., Karl, A. and Holzapfel, S.R. (2001), “Psychophysiological reactivity of aggressive drivers: an exploratory study”, Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, Vol. 26 No. 2, pp. 95-116.

Markkula, G., Engstrom, J., Lodin, J., Bargman, J. and Victor, T. (2016), “A farewell to brake reaction times? Kinematics-dependent brake response in naturalistic rear-end emergencies”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 95, pp. 209-226.

Mishra, M. and Kumar, A. (2021), “ADAS technology: a review on challenges, legal risk mitigation and solutions”, Autonomous Driving and Advanced Driver-Assistance Systems (ADAS), pp. 401-408.

NBSC, National Bureau of Statistics of China (2021), available at: https://data.stats.gov.cn/easyquery.htm?cn=C01&zb=A0S0D01&sj=2021 (accessed 10 June 2022).

NHTSA, the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (2004), www.volpe.dot.gov/safety-management-and-human-factors/surface-transportation-human-factors/safety-vehicle-using. (accessed 14 October 2019).

NHTSA, the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (2019), “Distracted driving in fatal crashes, 2017”, Traffic Safety Facts Research Note. Report No. DOT-HS-812-700, Washington, D.C.

Nowosielski, R.J., Trick, L.M. and Toxopeus, R. (2018), “Good distractions: testing the effects of listening to an audiobook on driving performance in simple and complex road environments”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 111, pp. 202-209.

NSC, the National Safety Council (2012), “Understanding the distracted brain: why driving while using hands-free cell phones is risky behavior”, White paper, available at: distracteddriving.nsc.org (accessed 4 October 2019).

Peng, Y. and Boyle, L.N. (2012), “Commercial driver factors in run-off-road crashes”, Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board, Vol. 2281 No. 1, pp. 128-132.

PLUX (2019), “PLUX the biosignalsplux professional”, https://plux.info/12-biosignalsplux, (accessed 10 October 2019).

Qi, Y., Vennu, R. and Pokhrel, R. (2020), “Distracted driving: a literature review”, Illinois Center for Transportation/Illinois Department of Transportation.

Qin, L., Li, Z.R., Chen, Z., Bill, M.A. and Noyce, D.A. (2019), “Understanding driver distractions in fatal crashes: an exploratory empirical analysis”, Journal of Safety Research, Vol. 69, pp. 23-31.

Sacha, J. (2014), “Interaction between heart rate and heart rate variability”, Annals of Noninvasive Electrocardiology, Vol. 19 No. 3, pp. 207-216.

Strayer, D.L. and Johnston, W.A. (2001), “Driven to distraction: dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular telephone”, Psychological Science, Vol. 12 No. 6, pp. 462-466, doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00386.

Summala, H. (2000), “Brake reaction times and driver behavior analysis”, Transportation Human Factors, Vol. 2 No. 3, pp. 217-226.

Vetter, P., Leicht, L., Leonhardt, S. and Teichmann, D. (2017), “Integration of an electromagnetic coupled sensor into a driver seat for vital sign monitoring: initial insight”, In Proceedings of 2017 IEEE International Conference on Vehicular Electronics and Safety, Vienna, Austria, pp. 185-190.

Wijayaratna, K.P., Cunningham, M.L., Regan, M.A., Jian, S., Chand, S. and Dixit, V.V. (2019), “Mobile phone conversation distraction: understanding differences in impact between simulator and naturalistic driving studies”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 129, pp. 108-118.

Wu, J. and Xu, H. (2018), “The influence of road familiarity on distracted driving activities and driving operation using naturalistic driving study data”, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, Vol. 52, pp. 75-85, doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2017.11.018.

Xu, J. and Lin, Y. (2018), “Impact of distracted drivers on individual and group behavior of following vehicles: a networked multi-driver simulator study”, Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board, Vol. 2672 No. 37, pp. 45-54.

Xue, Q., Gao, K., Xing, Y., Lu, J. and Qu, X. (2021), “A context-aware framework for risky driving behavior evaluation based on trajectory data”, IEEE Intelligent Transportation Systems Magazine, pp. 2-15.

Zeeb, K., Buchner, A. and Schrauf, M. (2016), “Is take-over time all that matters? The impact of visual-cognitive load on driver take-over quality after conditionally automated driving”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 92, pp. 230-239.

Zhang, F., Mehrotra, S. and Roberts, S.C. (2019), “Driving distracted with friends: effect of passengers and driver distraction on young drivers behavior”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 132, pp. 105-246.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Zhigang Xu and Runmin Wang, from the Connected and Automated Vehicle Test (CAVTest) laboratory of Chang’an University, for their assistance in the data collection process.

Funding: National Natural Science Foundation of China (52002031). National Natural Science Foundation of China (52172325). Key Research and Development Project of China (2021YFB1600104). Key Research and Development Project of Shaanxi Province (2019GY-070). Key Research and Development Project of Shaanxi Province (2020GY-027). National Key R&D Program of China (2019YFE0108300). Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (300102242902).

Corresponding author

Li Zhao can be contacted at: lizhao@unl.edu

Related articles