No trust, no use: how young retail investors build initial trust in financial robo-advisors

Mustafa Nourallah (Department of Economics, Geography, Law and Tourism, Centre for Research on Economic Relations, Mid Sweden University, Sundsvall, Sweden)
Peter Öhman (Department of Economics, Geography, Law and Tourism, Centre for Research on Economic Relations, Mid Sweden University, Sundsvall, Sweden)
Muslim Amin (Department of Marketing Strategy and Innovation, Sunway University Business School, Sunway University, Subang Jaya, Malaysia)

Journal of Financial Reporting and Accounting

ISSN: 1985-2517

Article publication date: 25 July 2022

Issue publication date: 21 March 2023

3201

Abstract

Purpose

The purpose of this study is to describe and analyse the effect of a set of determinants on initial trust and behavioural intention to use financial robo-advisors (FRAs).

Design/methodology/approach

The theory of perceived risk and the behavioural finance paradigm were used to develop a conceptual model of retail investors’ initial trust in FRAs. Data collected from 554 young retail investors (YRIs) from Sweden and Malaysia were analysed using structural equation modelling.

Findings

The results of this study indicate that the amount of public information, social media information-seeking and a rational decision style are significantly related to initial trust in FRAs, which in turn is significantly and positively related to the behavioural intention to use this technology. However, none of the risks under study significantly affect the initial trust in FRAs.

Practical implications

Information is vital to inducing YRIs to rely on FRAs, so the more public and social media information is available, the higher their intention to use this technology. However, YRIs vary in decision style, and the results suggest implementing a more sophisticated system than the current “one-size-fits-all” approach to YRI behaviour.

Originality/value

The empirical-based model enhances the knowledge of the initial phase of trust-building, when YRIs lack sufficient experience of FRAs. By collecting data from two countries, the study’s novel conclusions may help in developing effective FRA services for the youth segment.

Keywords

Citation

Nourallah, M., Öhman, P. and Amin, M. (2023), "No trust, no use: how young retail investors build initial trust in financial robo-advisors", Journal of Financial Reporting and Accounting, Vol. 21 No. 1, pp. 60-82. https://doi.org/10.1108/JFRA-12-2021-0451

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2022, Mustafa Nourallah, Peter Öhman and Muslim Amin.

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence maybe seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcodev


1. Introduction

Non-professional investors buy and sell securities through financial advisors, sharing their experiences to maximize the value of their investments (Kinder, 2015). Relying on human advisors in financial decision-making could be beneficial but exposes retail investors to the risk of receiving deceptive advice (D’Acunto et al., 2019). In fact, retail investors who fear being victimized by biased financial advice tend to rely on financial robo-advisors (FRAs), that is, cutting-edge financial technology (FinTech) solutions offering affordable automated services (Brenner and Meyll, 2020). At the same time, the ongoing shift from “human-to-human” to “human-to-machine” relationships in the financial sector raises questions as to how trust can be built in a machine-based environment (Goldstein et al., 2019; Kostovetsky, 2016). The initial phase of such trust-building seems particularly critical, because it will likely determine the intention to use FRAs (Cheng et al., 2019).

Relying on FRAs exposes retail investors to various risks. FRAs normally use an online questionnaire to evaluate their risk tolerance, that is, their risk profile, and this could lead investors with different risk profiles to be treated similarly (European Commission, 2018). In the same vein, Scherer (2017, p. 50) reported that “prominent German robo-advisor firms (Vaamo, Liqid, Quirion, and Scalable Capital) … fail to consider the influence of human capital, financial assets, financial liabilities, or real estate on recommended asset allocations”. This indicates that FRA questionnaires lack sophisticated questions that could correctly identify individual risk profiles.

Previous studies have emphasized the importance of information in building the relationship between retail investors and human financial advisors (Kostovetsky, 2016), and this seems equally relevant in the FRA context (Jung et al., 2018a). The supply of public information is essential in determining initial trust in FinTech solutions (McKnight et al., 1998), and social media are nowadays an essential source of information regarding financial decision-making (Florendo and Estelami, 2019; TIAA, 2021). The degree to which retail investors can rely on FRAs is also subject to individuals’ decision styles. In contrast to a rational decision style, those who are used to making intuitive decisions tend to use FRAs without in-depth investigations of the main features of this technology (Hamilton et al., 2016).

Retail investors’ perceptions of risk, access to available information and their decision style may differ depending on their experience or age (Koestner et al., 2017; Stålnacke, 2019). Consequently, previous studies divide retail investors into various categories. This research focuses on young retail investors (YRIs), that is, individuals 18–29 years old. Despite their modest portfolios, they represent a promising market for the financial sector.

FRAs offer impartial financial advice anytime and anywhere at an affordable price and with almost no restrictions in terms of the minimum invested amount. For example, an account with only US$50 could be opened through most FRAs with annual fees of under 1% of the value of the investor’s portfolio. Such advantages enable this technology to target the less profitable segment of the financial market (Anagnostopoulos, 2018; Brenner and Meyll, 2020). Previous studies have raised concerns about FRAs in terms of the possibility of designing algorithms that guide YRIs to invest in specific assets (European Commission, 2018) and whether FRAs are intended to be used without any personal contact (Jung et al., 2018a). Another concern is whether the “one-size-fits-all” FRA approach is sufficient regardless of the investor’s decision style (Abraham et al., 2019). However, few empirical investigations have focused on how retail investors build trust in FRAs (Gan et al., 2021).

This study describes and analyses the relationship between perceived risks (i.e. financial, performance, security and privacy and social risks), information (i.e. amount of public information and social media information-seeking) and decision style (i.e. rational vs intuitive), on one hand, and YRIs’ initial trust in FRAs, on the other. It also highlights the relationship between initial trust and behavioural intention to use FRAs and compares YRIs in different locations.

The study enriches our knowledge of YRIs’ preferences related to FRAs based on their experience of various cultural conditions (Hofstede, 2001). Regarding ongoing development in the FinTech area, Sweden and Malaysia have the necessary prerequisites to enable use of FRAs. Both countries have well-developed technological facilities and are on their way towards being cashless societies (Nourallah and Öhman, 2021). They also represent hubs of FinTech solutions, including FRAs. Recent figures indicate that the amount of assets under management in FRAs has increased in Sweden and Malaysia from US$143 and 12.1m in 2017, respectively, to US$473 and 62m in 2019 and is expected to reach approximately US$1,894 and 294m in 2023 (Statista, 2019a, 2019b). Despite these similarities, it is important to follow the recommendations of Ameen et al. (2021) and investigate whether cultural differences may influence YRIs’ perceptions of FRAs. Conducting research on Swedish and Malaysian YRIs may also help improve the development of FRAs in these highly technology-based parts of the world.

The rest of the paper is structured as follows: Section 2 introduces the frame of reference, Section 3 presents the hypotheses and research model and Section 4 discusses the methods used. Section 5 reports the data analysis and the empirical results, and Section 6 presents the concluding remarks, including a discussion, implications, limitations and suggestions for further research.

2. Frame of reference

2.1 Theory of perceived risk and financial robo-advisors

The theory of perceived risk views individuals’ behaviour in terms of their interior estimates of uncertainty and of the negative consequences of given situations (Bauer, 1960). According to Mitchell (1999, p. 168), perceived risk represents the “subjectively-determined expectation of loss; the greater the probability of this loss, the greater the risk thought to exist for an individual”.

Jacoby and Kaplan (1972) presented a general five-dimension scale of perceived risk, comprising financial, performance, physical, psychological and social risks. More recently, Chen (2013) found that financial, performance, time, psychological and privacy risks are related to perceived risk. In an empirical investigation of how the young generation perceives the risk of mobile payment, Akturan and Tezcan (2012) found that social and performance risks are significantly related to total perceived risk.

While some studies have used one scale consisting of several dimensions (Chen, 2013), others have used one-item scales (Alalwan et al., 2016). Besides these two approaches, FinTech studies (Lee, 2009) have used several independent constructs of perceived risk, finding that types of perceived risk vary based on the service offered (Featherman and Pavlou, 2003).

To determine the types of perceived risk that could be considered by YRIs, the FRA literature was consulted. It showed, first, that there is no guarantee that the use of FRAs will lead to desirable financial outcomes, that is, financial risk does exist (Abraham et al., 2019). Second, FRA performance influences the extent to which YRIs can benefit from using this technology. Errors or difficulties associated with using FRAs are examples of performance risk (Jung et al., 2018a). In this regard, Beketov et al. (2018) investigated 219 FRAs globally and found that only 73 disclosed information regarding the asset allocation method. Their study prompted inquiries as to why only one-third of the studied FRAs were transparent about this matter. Third, privacy and security matters represent a significant issue, as this technology demands that YRIs provide sensitive financial information. Berg et al. (2020) reported that even general information, for example, about the type of device (i.e. digital footprint), could reveal important information about individuals. Fourth, issues such as peers’ opinions emphasize a certain type of perceived risk: namely, social risk. Ayton et al. (2020, p. 1) argued that an “extensive and burgeoning body of research reveals that factors associated with the social environment play a critical role”. Overall and in line with the theory of perceived risk, it can be argued that financial, performance, privacy and security and social risks might influence YRIs’ initial trust in FRAs.

2.2 The behavioural finance paradigm and financial robo-advisors

The behavioural finance paradigm concerns how individuals behave in financial decision-making processes (Thaler, 1993). In contrast to traditional finance assumptions, in which individuals are seen as rational and able to maximize their utility (Andrikopoulos and Vagenas-Nanos, 2017), the behavioural finance paradigm claims that individuals are limited in their mental ability (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979), have bounded rationality (Simon, 1991) and are often influenced by various cognitive biases (Kahneman, 2011). In this regard, Baker and Nofsinger (2010, p. 3) argued that “the thinking process does not work like a computer. Instead, the human brain often processes information using shortcuts and emotional filters. These processes influence financial decision-makers such that people often act in a seemingly irrational manner, routinely violate traditional concepts of risk aversion, and make predictable errors in their forecasts”.

The behavioural finance paradigm incorporates finance theory and psychology to identify solutions to financial problems, especially those that traditional finance seems unable to explain (Hirshleifer, 2015). DeBondt et al. (2010) argued that the contribution of psychology to the behavioural finance paradigm has led to three streams of research: first, emotional finance, that is, a paradigm that uses emotional responses and investigates investment activity by incorporating emotions in financial decision-making (Andrikopoulos and Vagenas-Nanos, 2017); second, social finance, that is, a paradigm that uses social psychology and considers the influence of social norms, moral attitudes, religions and ideologies on individuals’ financial behaviour (Hirshleifer, 2015); and third, cognitive finance, that is, a paradigm that uses cognitive psychology and focuses on decision style in the financial context of spending, saving and investing (Otto, 2007). The last stream of research, that is, cognitive finance, emphasizes the role of information and decision style in the financial decision-making of retail investors, which is the focus of this study.

The information delivered to individuals has a significant role in the reliance on FRAs (Litterscheidt and Streich, 2020). In the initial phase of dealing with FRAs, YRIs lack sufficient information about this technology but might access public information. An important source of such information is the methodology section of the FRA’s website (or mobile application), which provides details about the asset allocation method used. Such information is important, as it explains how the FRA works. However, many FRAs do not disclose the method used for asset allocation (Beketov et al., 2018). Previous results have further indicated that information obtained from social media can influence retail investors’ decisions (Tan and Tas, 2021).

Individuals’ decision styles perform a vital role in their financial decision-making. Kahneman (2011) distinguished between two decision styles. The first is a fast style, or an intuitive style, which is used by individuals who make decisions emotionally, such as by using personal feelings to make a move in a chess game. The second is a slow style, or a rational style, which is used by individuals who likely follow a logical approach, such as by making calculations before making a decision.

Taken together and in line with the behavioural finance paradigm, information (i.e. amount of public information and social media information-seeking) and decision style (i.e. rational or intuitive) might influence YRIs’ initial trust in FRAs.

2.3 Initial trust and behavioural intention to use financial robo-advisors

The literature on trust reveals two important things. First, there is a lack of agreement on how to characterize trust (Schoorman et al., 2007). Economists are likely to use trust in a calculative sense based on cost and benefit theory. Psychologists focus on the attributes of trustors and trustees, and sociologists argue that trust is a phenomenon that evolves in individual relationships (Rousseau et al., 1998). Second, previous studies agree on the dynamic nature of trust, meaning that trust development starts from a certain level and can later increase or decrease (Schoorman et al., 2007). Initial trust refers to the trust-building phase in which two parties meet or interact without prior experience or knowledge of each other, and they may accept vulnerability to fulfil their wants and needs (McKnight et al., 1998).

Rousseau et al. (1998) asserted that trust is not a behaviour as such but an underlying psychological state that could cause choice behaviour. Fishbein and Ajzen (1975) defined behavioural intention as a subjective estimate of the probability of a particular behaviour, and Zeithaml et al. (1996) argued that behavioural intention to use concerns whether individuals, such as users of a certain technology, will remain with or leave the service provider. Similarly, previous studies have found a relationship between initial trust and intention to use and noted that the latter concept is dominant in the FinTech context (Alalwan et al., 2016; Tam and Oliveira, 2017).

In the FinTech context, trust determines the adoption of FinTech solutions (Jüngera and Mietznerb, 2020), and the initial phase of developing trust (i.e. initial trust) is equally important (Bhatia et al., 2020). Initial trust represents the willingness to trust a party without previous knowledge and is built on the stimuli provided by FinTech solutions (Lee and Kim, 2020) or their reputation (Bhatia et al., 2020). Initial trust is critical, as it can lead individuals to continue trusting FRAs and determines whether they will allocate money to invest while relying on this technology (Jung et al., 2018a, 2018b).

3. Hypothesis development and research model

Financial risk can be described as the chance of someone suffering from significant losses or not achieving expected return (Vlaev et al., 2009). Previous studies have concluded that financial risk influences trust-building. Lee (2009) reported that financial risk is negatively related to use of e-banking, and Chiu et al. (2016) found that perceived cost has a direct relationship with initial trust. Like anyone else, YRIs try to avoid risks when they make investments using securities brokerages. If they think that using FRAs is risky, then they will not trust them. Current FRA practices attempt to minimize financial risk through “risk profiling”, that is, matching a potential user’s characteristics with a predesigned risk profile. Such profiling will likely address YRIs’ perceptions of the risk of FRAs, influencing their initial trust in this technology (McKnight et al., 1998). Although recent results indicate that financial risk could have a limited effect on retail investors because investment always carries such risk (Chong et al., 2021), the current study hypothesizes that:

H1.

The higher the financial risk, the lower the initial trust in financial robo-advisors.

Performance risk can be described as the possibility that a service provider does not operate in the way individuals expect or fails to deliver the desired benefit (Lee, 2009). In the FRA context, several algorithms are used to analyse YRIs’ answers in online questionnaires and then suggest various investment portfolios in terms of risk and return (Bhatia et al., 2020). These suggested portfolios largely determine the risk YRIs might face and the expected return on their investments (Brenner and Meyll, 2020). The better the FRA performance, the more appropriate portfolio suggestions the YRIs will get. In contrast, inconsistent FRA performance guides YRIs to make bad decisions.

Lee (2009) reported that performance risk is negatively related to the attitude towards using E-banking, and Chiu et al. (2016) suggested that the quality of infrastructure is directly related to initial trust. In addition, Seiler and Fanenbruck (2021) found no significant relationship between ease of use and the intention to use FRAs. Based on these findings, it can be argued that FRA performance will likely influence initial trust, so it is hypothesized that:

H2.

The higher the performance risk, the lower the initial trust in financial robo-advisors.

Security and privacy risk can be described as the harms and threats that mitigate service safety and the individuals’ concerns about their personal information (Al-Khalaf and Choe, 2020). This risk seems to concern trust in financial service providers (Lee, 2009). Previous studies have reported a negative relationship between privacy and security risk, on one hand, and initial trust, on the other (Chiu et al., 2016). Similarly, Zhou (2012) found empirical evidence that e-banking’s assurance, confidence and robust attributes influence initial trust. In contrast, Amirtha et al. (2021) reported an insignificant relationship between privacy risk and behavioural intention to use.

When using FRAs, YRIs need to answer several personal questions regarding their bank accounts and income (Brenner and Meyll, 2020). Although FRAs consider security and privacy to protect investors (Gan et al., 2021), problems might arise and mishandling of sensitive information could negatively affect initial trust in this technology. Thus, in a relationship like the one between YRIs and FRAs, security and privacy risk will likely determine the initial trust in FRAs. This study hypothesizes that:

H3.

The higher the security and privacy risk, the lower the initial trust in financial robo-advisors.

Retail investors are subject to the influence of peer opinions, including advise from friends and family members (Stålnacke, 2019). Because of YRIs’ strong family relationships (Gudmunson et al., 2016), family support and related advice will likely affect their trust in FRAs. Peers’ opinions about a particular FinTech solution might determine retail investors’ decisions to use this solution (Gomber et al., 2017). For example, YRIs consider peers’ opinions expressed via social media platforms when determining what FRAs they might use (TIAA, 2021). Alalwan et al. (2016) reported that social risk directly relates to the adoption of mobile banking, and Tandon et al. (2018) argued that this risk decreases satisfaction. In the e-banking context, Kaabachi et al. (2019) reported that social influence affects initial trust. Although studies like that of Dharmesti et al. (2021) have found that social motives will reduce the purchase intentions of youth, most arguments point to the following hypothesis:

H4.

The higher the social risk, the lower the initial trust in financial robo-advisors.

FRAs disseminate public information to inform YRIs about their services, benefits, etc. This type of information represents unfiltered financial information (Stålnacke, 2019), for example, regarding the methodology used to match the characteristics of a potential user with a predesigned risk profile. Learning about FRA services and how to use them increases the trust in FRAs (Chiu et al., 2016). Getting information issued by regulated parties such as FRAs helps YRIs feel that they have the information they need. In the e-banking context, Kaabachi et al. (2019) reported that information provided to individuals has a direct and positive relationship with the initial trust. Previous studies have also found that the quality of the information provided determines the initial trust (Zhou, 2012). Thus, the hypothesis is as follows:

H5.

The higher amount of public information young retail investors receive, the higher their initial trust in financial robo-advisors.

Social media information-seeking is defined as “the extent to which news shared in social media can provide users with relevant and timely information” (Lee and Ma, 2012, p. 336). Although there are questions about the reliability of this information source (Florendo and Estelami, 2019), the young generation uses social media to share information and stay up to date on “mega-trends” (Yoshida et al., 2018). A recent example is the sharing of opinions via the Reddit social media platform, which helped build YRIs’ initial trust in the Robinhood FRA. Al-Khalaf and Choe (2020) found that social media influences trust in the e-commerce context, and Laroche et al. (2012) reported that individuals who seek information in social media communities will likely trust the provider. Pentina et al. (2013) stated that information aligns individuals’ preferences directly with trust. Hence, the current study hypothesizes that:

H6.

The greater the social media information-seeking conducted by young retail investors, the higher their initial trust in financial robo-advisors.

An individual’s style of perceiving, processing and responding in decision-making situations represents a stable personal pattern (Cosenza et al., 2019). A rational decision style refers to individuals who conduct a comprehensive search for information and a systematic assessment of all potential choices. In contrast, an intuitive decision style refers to individuals who use a quick decision-making process that relies on hunches and feelings (Hamilton et al., 2017).

Gambetti and Giusberti (2019) reported a positive relationship between the rational but not the intuitive, decision-making style and the decision to invest in various stocks. Thus, the decision style helps in identifying decision-making-related differences among individuals (Kahneman, 2011), and YRIs’ decision styles might affect their behaviour. For example, YRIs with a rational decision style often read the methodology section provided by FRAs to know more about the services. A contradictory scenario might happen with YRIs using an intuitive decision style. Thus, the current study hypothesizes that:

H7.

There is a significant relationship between a rational decision style and initial trust in financial robo-advisors.

H8.

There is no significant relationship between an intuitive decision style and initial trust in financial robo-advisors.

As indicated, individuals show a tendency to act in a certain way based on their initial trust in a situation (McKnight et al., 1998). This means that trust determines behaviour (Rousseau et al., 1998) and influences the intention to act (Jung et al., 2018b). Previous studies have suggested that initial trust plays an essential role in enhancing behavioural intention (Kaabachi et al., 2019), and Ofori et al. (2018) found a significant relationship between trust and behavioural intention to use e-commerce services. In this context, YRIs who trust FRAs seem more likely to continue to use the technology. The current study hypothesizes the following:

H9.

The higher the initial trust in financial robo-advisors, the higher the behavioural intention to use financial robo-advisors.

4. Methods

4.1 Measurement development

Items adapted from previous studies and forming a preliminary questionnaire were presented at an academic seminar. Based on the received comments, English and Swedish versions were sent to three Swedish YRIs and two experts in the field of the study to check their clarity and readability. After additional language revisions, the questionnaire was sent to five YRIs in each of Sweden and Malaysia. Their feedback helped us avoid minor issues related to readability.

The final questionnaire was based on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = “strongly disagree” to 7 = “strongly agree”. The appendix shows the items included in the questionnaire and operationalizations, concepts and references. The background variables were age, gender, preferred device used for electronic financial transactions, FRA experience and investment experience.

4.2 Sample, data collection and descriptive statistics

Individuals who might use FRAs are likely to be young (US Financial Industry Regulatory Authority, 2016), to have modest income and wealth (Fulk et al., 2018) and to have limited investment experience (Welch, 2022). Without having built trust towards FRAs yet, these individuals can adequately respond to questions on initial trust. Thus, the questionnaire was distributed to 202 university students in Sweden and 352 university students in Malaysia, of whom 116 (57% response rate) and 280 (86% response rate) submitted completed questionnaires. The respondents studied business administration, finance and economics and engineering. Issues related to the confidentiality of the collected data and data storage were taken into consideration, and respondents over 29 years old were eliminated from the sample.

Table 1 shows that the Swedish respondents were relatively balanced between the two age groups (i.e. 18–23 and 24–29 years old), while most Malaysian respondents belonged to the younger subgroup. Regarding gender, Swedish respondents included a rather equal number of males and females, while the Malaysian ones were mainly females. The responses revealed that most respondents in both countries used mobiles to conduct electronic financial transactions and lacked prior experience of FRAs. In Sweden and Malaysia, only 12.9% and 3.2% of the respondents conducted more than 36 transactions a year, respectively.

5. Data analysis and empirical results

Smart PLS software, version 3.0, developed by Ringle et al. (2005) was used to run the measurement and structural models. According to Hair et al. (2019), partial least squares-structural equation modelling is a causal-predictive technique highlighting prediction in estimating statistical models, and there are several reasons for applying such a procedure: sample size, distributional assumptions and statistical power.

5.1 Measurement model

Following Hair et al. (2019), internal consistency reliability, convergent validity and discriminant validity were calculated in the measurement model. Regarding internal consistency reliability, Cronbach’s alpha ranged from 0.817 to 0.951. Regarding convergent validity, the factor loadings, composite reliability (CR) and average variance extracted (AVE) were calculated for each construct. As shown in Table 2, the ranges of the factor loadings, CR and AVE indicate that all constructs reached recommended levels (cf. Anderson and Gerbing, 1988).

Regarding discriminant validity, Fornell and Larcker’s (1981) procedure and the heterotrait–monotrait (HTMT) ratio of correlations (Henseler et al., 2016) were used. Fornell and Larcker results in Table 3 indicate that the square root of the AVE between each pair of constructs was higher than the correlation estimated between constructs, establishing acceptable discriminant validity. The HTMT ratio of correlations clarifies that all values were lower than the recommended level of 0.85 (Hair et al., 2019), indicating satisfactory discriminant validity.

5.2 Structural model

The path coefficient (β), coefficient of determination (R2) and effect size (f2) are reported in the structural model. Using a bootstrapping procedure with a resampling of 5,000, the path estimates and t-statistics were calculated for the hypothesized interactions (Figure 1). As shown in Table 4, the relationships between financial risk, performance risk and security and privacy risk, on one hand, on initial trust, on the other, are not significant. Accordingly, H1, H2 and H3 are not supported. The significant relationship between social risk is in the opposite direction in relation to the hypothesis, meaning that H4 is also rejected. The relationships between amount of public information, social media information-seeking and rational decision style, respectively, and initial trust are significant and in the direction suggested by H5, H6 and H7. The relationship between intuitive decision style and initial trust is insignificant, so H8 is supported. As initial trust is significantly and positively related to behavioural intention to use FRAs, H9 is supported. The R2 values in Table 4 show that the initial trust is explained by 61.9% of the determining variables under study and that behavioural intention is explained by 37.6% of the initial trust. The f2 values indicate each construct’s small, medium and large effects.

5.3 Multi-group analysis

The multi-group data analysis (MGA) was conducted to compare the Swedish (n = 116) and Malaysian (n = 280) respondents. Following Henseler et al. (2016), the procedure to determine the measurement invariance of composites procedure was performed in three steps: configural invariance assessment, establishment of compositional invariance assessment and assessment of equal means and variances. The partial measurement invariance of the two groups was generated as a requirement for comparing and interpreting the MGA group-specific differences in the partial least squares-structural equation modelling results. As shown in Table 5, there are no significant differences between the two groups in the determinants under study regarding initial trust. However, regarding initial trust and behavioural intention to use FRAs, there is a significant difference between the Swedish and Malaysian YRIs.

6. Concluding remarks

6.1 Discussion

The theory of perceived risk suggests various types of risk that could influence initial trust in FRAs. However, this study concludes that none of the risks under study significantly affects the initial trust in FRAs. Although the perceived risk results are in contrast to what was hypothesized, they confirm the work of Chong et al. (2021), who found no significant relationship between YRIs’ perceived risk and the intention to adopt mobile stock trading. One possible explanation for the lack of relationships is that FRAs have to some extent customized their services (Bhatia et al., 2020; Jung et al., 2018b). In line with this, Wu and Gao (2021, p. 273) found that “customized features were beneficial to decrease users’ perceived risk of a robo-advisor”. It can also be argued that, because of YRIs’ modest portfolios (Fulk et al., 2018), the role of financial risk seems limited. Moreover, because of YRIs’ good technological skills and their potential lack of trust in human financial advisors, they are less likely to face performance-related issues when using FRAs. This could be related to the findings of Seiler and Fanenbruck (2021), who reported no significant relationship between ease of use of FRAs and the intention to use this technology.

It seems as though YRIs have a good capacity to deal with technical issues concerning security and privacy. For example, they are aware of the need to update their operating system frequently and download robust antivirus software. Previous studies have argued that the members of the young generation behave differently in different application contexts, prioritizing things differently when using applications for instrumental purposes, such as investment, than for emotional and social purposes (Nourallah, 2022). In other words, YRIs might be less worried about security and privacy issues when they invest their modest portfolio than when they use digital social media apps, which could convey information about their life events. Considering social risk, this result indicates a significant positive relationship with initial trust. This result echoes the work of Dharmesti et al. (2021), who found that social motives negatively affect the online purchase intentions of the young generation. One possible explanation is that youths like to experience adventure, so peers’ concerns about emerging technologies may not deter YRIs but rather encourage them to try such technologies.

In line with the behavioural finance paradigm, this study supports the hypothesized role of information in addressing YRIs’ initial trust in FRAs. The amount of public information is essential for retail investors (Stålnacke, 2019), making them more familiar with the financial services and possibly strengthening both their intention to use and actual use of app technology (Rezaei et al., 2016). Bapat (2020) found that public information – such as interest rate, credit score and investment details – is important in financial planning. The more information the financial service providers deliver, the higher the initial trust will be (Kaabachi et al., 2019). This supports Chiu et al. (2016), who argued that acquiring information about how to use FinTech solutions is vital to building trust in these technologies. In line with Dharmesti et al. (2021), the current results also suggest that YRIs tend to search for information on various online platforms such as social media apps. According to Shaheen et al. (2020), information such as online reviews seems to be helpful in building trust among members of the younger generation.

The results support the hypothesized relationships between the rational and intuitive decision styles, respectively, and initial trust, confirming the results of Gambetti and Giusberti (2019). It is no surprise that YRIs with a rational decision style are more likely to feel initial trust in FRAs than are their peers with an intuitive decision style. Rational decision style individuals tend to adopt analytical decision strategies (Zhu et al., 2021) and will likely use information in making decisions (Hamilton et al., 2017). For example, in contrast to YRIs with an intuitive decision style, those with a rational decision style search for available information in FRA websites or apps to build trust in this technology.

As hypothesized, there is a positive and significant relationship between initial trust in FRAs and the intention to use this technology, confirming the work of Alkraiji and Ameen (2021), who emphasized the role of trust in stimulating the young generations’ intention to use digital platforms.

It appears that YRIs’ similar lifestyles – listening to music via Spotify, watching movies via Netflix, chatting via WhatsApp, etc. – have led the ones in this generation to perceive the relationships between the determinants of initial trust and initial trust similarly. The results of the MGA analysis indicate that the only significant difference between the Swedish and Malaysian groups concerns the relationship between initial trust and behavioural intention to use FRAs. The Malaysian group has a significantly higher Beta of 0.796 > 0.479, indicating that the action plans seem to differ depending on the YRIs’ locations. Gan et al. (2021, p. 12) reported that in Malaysia, rigorously restricted personal contact during the COVID-19 pandemic encouraged individuals to trust FRAs for financial and wealth management and argued that “the augmented trust then led to a stronger intention to adopt FRAs among Malaysians”. The questionnaires were sent to YRIs during the lockdown period (between October 2020 and February 2021), and the restrictions, including the lack of opportunities to contact human financial advisors, may have increased the Malaysians’ intention to use FRAs. The restrictions in Sweden were not as strict during the pandemic as they were in Malaysia.

6.2 Theoretical implications

Based on the theory of perceived risk, the behavioural finance paradigm and previous studies of initial trust, this study developed a conceptual model. The suggested model provides novel insights into YRIs and FRAs and enhances our knowledge of the initial phase of trust-building when YRIs still lack sufficient experience of FRAs. The empirical investigation emphasizes the role that public and social media information could have in building trust in FRAs. This contributes to the literature on emerging technology in the FinTech context and indicates that the two information variables are essential in studying the trust-building phase. Thus, the higher the availability of information about a certain FinTech solution, the higher the initial trust that could form, and, as a result, individuals will likely have a higher intention to use this technology, that is, information → initial trust → behavioural intention to use.

Moreover, the literature on retail investing has more or less ignored personal characteristics such as decision style (Gambetti and Giusberti, 2019), instead prioritizing other social-economic factors such as gender and income. This study emphasizes that decision style could reveal relatively more about the behaviour of retail investors in the FinTech context than could many other factors.

6.3 Managerial implications

The financial sector, including wealth management and financial advice, has witnessed the rise of advanced technology that allows new parties, such as FinTech companies, to offer digital services (Nourallah and Öhman, 2021). Among these parties, FRAs have attracted the attention of YRIs (Brenner and Meyll, 2020). To achieve initial trust, FRAs could deliver more public information, illustrating how risk profiles are constructed and describing the method used for asset allocation. It is also important to make information available via social media sources. Regarding the issue of the credibility of social media sources, FRAs would benefit from sharing verified information via these sources.

Most FRAs strive to attract investors through “easy to complete” questionnaires. However, this “one-size-fits-all” approach has been criticized (Abraham et al., 2019; Scherer, 2017), and it is recommended that FRAs should use a more sophisticated system to correctly identify and categorize YRI behaviour. This study highlights the need to customize the available information in a way that suits the decision styles of retail investors and enables them to obtain knowledge about the most critical matters, such as asset allocation processes.

6.4 Limitation and suggestions for future research

This study collected and analysed data on university students in two countries having good technological infrastructure. Future studies should select respondents with more diverse educational backgrounds and geographical locations. As suggested by Ameen et al. (2021), future research could investigate the moderating role of cultural dimensions. As most of the respondents lacked experience in using technology to build their investment portfolios, this study did not investigate potential differences between less- and more-experienced retail investors. Future studies could, therefore, consider making such comparisons. Future studies could also apply other theories, such as the unified theory of acceptance and use of technology, to further develop the empirically based model of initial trust in FRAs presented here. Our study failed to demonstrate the expected influences of financial, performance, security and privacy and social risks, indicating a need for further studies of these factors.

Figures

Results for structural model of initial trust in financial robo-advisors (dotted lines indicate non-significant results and dashed lines indicate significant results)

Figure 1.

Results for structural model of initial trust in financial robo-advisors (dotted lines indicate non-significant results and dashed lines indicate significant results)

Descriptive statistics

Sweden Malaysia
Age 18–23 years (55.2%)
24–29 years (44.8%)
18–23 years (95.7%)
24–29 years (4.3%)
Gender Female (47%)
Male (50%)
Prefer not to say (3%)
Female (60%)
Male (39.1%)
Prefer not to say (0.9%)
Which of the following devices do you use to conduct electronic financial transactions?* Mobile app (96.6%)
Computer or laptop (67.2%)
Smartwatch (1.7%)
Tablet (7.8%)
Other (0.9%)
Mobile app (86.1%)
Computer or laptop (52.9%)
Smartwatch (1.4%)
Tablet (6.4%)
Other (6.4%)
Do you have experience in using FRAs? No (87.9%)
Yes, less than 1 year (5.2%)
Yes, at least 1 year and less than 2 years (2.6%)
Yes, at least 2 years and less than 3 years (2.6%)
Yes, at least 3 years (1.7%)
No (94.6%)
Yes, less than 1 year (3.6%)
Yes, at least 1 year and less than 2 years (1.4%); Yes, at least 2 years and less than 3 years (0.4%); Yes, at least 3 years (0%)
How many investment transactions do you make during a year?§ No transactions (25%)
1–2 transaction(s) a year (10.4%)
3–10 transactions a year (16.4%)
11–35 transactions a year (35.3%)
More than 36 transactions a year (12.9%)
No transactions (61.2%)
1–2 transaction(s) a year (19.6%)
3–10 transactions a year (9.9%)
11–35 transactions a year (6.1%)
More than 36 transactions a year (3.2%)
Notes:

*More than one answer was allowed.

§

The ranges used were determined based on a phone interview with an expert in the investment of international markets

Construct validity

Constructs Items Loadings Cronbach’s
alpha
CR AVE Constructs Items Loadings Cronbach’s
alpha
CR AVE
Financial risk FR1 0.778 0.898 0.925 0.711 Social media information-seeking SMIS1 0.917 0.919 0.949 0.861
FR2 0.872 SMIS2 0.937
FR3 0.891 SMIS3 0.929
FR4 0.807 Rational decision style RDS1 0.891 0.951 0.962 0.837
FR5 0.863 RDS2 0.915
Performance risk PeR1 0.814 0.817 0.891 0.732 RDS3 0.931
PeR2 0.901 RDS4 0.922
PeR3 0.849 RDs5 0.913
Security and privacy risk SPR1 0.849 0.842 0.904 0.759 Intuitive decision style IDS1 0.845 0.893 0.922 0.702
SPR2 0.901 IDS2 0.860
SPR3 0.863 IDs3 0.855
Social risk SoR1 0.913 0.936 0.954 0.837 IDS4 0.846
SoR2 0.937 IDS5 0.779
SoR3 0.917 Initial trust IT1 0.887 0.913 0.939 0.793
SoR4 0.893 IT2 0.897
Amount of public information LPI1 0.906 0.951 0.965 0.872 IT3 0.887
LPI2 0.949 IT4 0.891
LPI3 0.964 Behavioural intention BIU1 0.942 0.915 0.946 0.855
LPI4 0.916 BIU2 0.935
BIU3 0.896
Notes:

CR = composite reliability; and AVE = average variance extracted

Discriminant validity

Constructs 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Fornell and Larcker’s procedure
1. Financial risk 0.843
2. Performance risk 0.817 0.855
3. Security and privacy risk 0.612 0.642 0.871
4. Social risk 0.591 0.59 0.518 0.915
5. Amount of public information 0.215 0.297 0.201 0.024 0.934
6. Social media information-seeking 0.344 0.362 0.417 0.563 0.362 0.928
7. Rational decision style 0.152 0.236 0.235 0.103 0.471 0.158 0.915
8. Intuitive decision style 0.196 0.206 0.285 0.376 0.152 0.489 0.257 0.838
9. Initial trust 0.344 0.424 0.383 0.368 0.62 0.622 0.412 0.381 0.890
10. Behavioural intention 0.259 0.295 0.299 0.56 0.183 0.572 0.103 0.462 0.613 0.925
Heterotrait–monotrait (HTMT) ratio of correlations
1. Financial risk
2. Performance risk 0.961
3. Security and privacy risk 0.698 0.767
4. Social risk 0.652 0.672 0.587
5. Amount of public information 0.229 0.34 0.219 0.045
6. Social media information-seeking 0.380 0.414 0.478 0.605 0.386
7. Rational decision style 0.159 0.270 0.257 0.113 0.494 0.164
8. Intuitive decision style 0.222 0.236 0.324 0.407 0.163 0.538 0.274
9. Initial trust 0.378 0.488 0.432 0.394 0.665 0.679 0.439 0.419
10. Behavioural intention 0.288 0.338 0.345 0.600 0.195 0.623 0.114 0.511 0.666

Structural model

Hypothesis β SD t-statistics p-values R2 f2 BCI LL BCI UL Decision
H1. Financial risk → Initial trust –0.068 0.079 0.860 0.390 0.619 0.004 –0.227 0.083 Not supported
H2. Performance risk → Initial trust 0.069 0.098 0.697 0.486 0.619 0.003 –0.127 0.255 Not supported
H3. Security and privacy risk → Initial trust 0.014 0.054 0.258 0.796 0.619 0.000 –0.091 0.116 Not supported
H4. Social risk → Initial trust 0.202 0.066 3.061 0.002 0.619 0.037 0.075 0.328 Not supported
H5. Amount of public information → Initial trust 0.423 0.056 7.585 0.000 0.619 0.279 0.306 0.518 Supported
H6. Social media information-seeking → Initial trust 0.298 0.068 4.369 0.000 0.619 0.109 0.167 0.427 Supported
H7. Rational Decision style → Initial trust 0.166 0.056 2.977 0.003 0.619 0.046 0.063 0.278 Supported
H8. Intuitive decision style –/> Initial trust 0.048 0.054 0.890 0.374 0.619 0.004 –0.059 0.158 Supported
H9. Initial trust → Behavioural intention 0.613 0.045 13.672 0.000 0.376 0.602 0.514 0.692 Supported
Notes:

→ = relationship; –/> = no relationship; β = the path coefficient; R2 = coefficient of determination; f2 = effect size; BCI LL = Beta coefficient lower level; BCI UL = Beta coefficient upper level; and p-value at 5%

Model comparison

Hypothesis Malaysian Swedish Malaysian–Swedish BCI LL BCI UL p-values Decision
β Β β differences
H1. Financial risk → Initial trust –0.082 –0.166 0.084 –0.370 0.331 0.657 Not supported
H2. Performance risk → Initial trust 0.109 0.056 0.053 –0.435 0.391 0.834 Not supported
H3. Security and privacy risk → Initial trust 0.042 0.073 –0.031 –0.246 0.259 0.815 Not supported
H4. Social risk → Initial trust 0.133 0.186 –0.053 –0.281 0.310 0.717 Not supported
H5. Amount of public information → Initial trust 0.362 0.397 –0.035 –0.222 0.259 0.798 Not supported
H6. Social media information-seeking → Initial trust 0.282 0.248 0.034 –0.297 0.333 0.855 Not supported
H7. Rational decision style → Initial trust 0.126 0.145 –0.019 –0.247 0.235 0.881 Not supported
H8. Intuitive decision style –/> Initial trust 0.076 0.116 –0.040 –0.237 0.235 0.735 Not supported
H8. Initial trust → Behavioural intention 0.796 0.479 0.317 –0.190 0.205 0.002 Supported
Notes:

→ = relationship; /> = no relationship; β = the path coefficient; BCI LL = Beta coefficient lower level; and BCI UL = Beta coefficient upper level

The questionnaire

Background variables
18–23 years 24–29 years Over 29 years
Gender
Female Male Prefer not to say
Which of the following devices do you use to conduct electronic financial transactions? (more than one choice can be made)
Mobile app Computer (or laptop) Smartwatch Tablet Other
Do you have experience in using financial robo-advisors (FRAs)?
No Yes, less than 1 year Yes, at least 1 year and less than 2 years
Yes, at least 2 years and less than 3 years Yes, at least 3 years
How many investment transactions do you make during a year?
0 1–2 3–10 11–35 More than 36
Risk, information, initial trust and behavioural intention to use variables
Item Operationalization Concept Source
FR1 I believe there would be problems with my financial transactions when using FRAs Financial risk Akturan and Tezcan (2012)
FR2 I believe that using FRAs is financially risky
FR3 I believe that there is a potential risk of large loss by using FRAs
FR4 I believe that there is a potential risk of returns below my initial target when using FRAs
FR5 I believe that I may lose money because of using FRAs
PeR1 I am concerned that the FRAs will not provide the level of benefits I expect Performance risk Akturan and Tezcan (2012), Chen (2013)
PeR2 The efficiency of FRAs differs from what I expect
PeR3 The performance of FRAs is inferior to that of human financial advisors
SPR1 I am worried that FRAs are not secure for making financial decisions Security and privacy risk Chen (2013), Tandon et al. (2018)
SPR2 My personal information (such as income) may be disclosed to others when using FRAs
SPR3 My private information may be subject to hacking issues when using FRAs
SoR1 I believe that using FRAs would not provide me with higher social status Social risk Akturan and Tezcan (2012), Tandon et al. (2018)
SoR2 I believe that I would not be held in higher esteem by my associates at work if I used FRAs
SoR3 The thought of using FRAs causes me concern because some friends would think I was just showing off
SoR4 I believe that using FRAs may result in disapproval from my community
LPI1 I believe in being totally informed about the range of products and services offered by FRAs Amount of public information Kaabachi et al. (2019)
LPI2 I believe in being totally informed about the benefits of using FRAs
LPI3 I believe in being totally informed about using FRAs
LPI4 I believe in being totally informed about security and privacy issues when using FRAs
SMIS1 Social media helps me to find useful information about FRAs Social media information-seeking Lee and Ma (2012)
SMIS2 Social media helps me to find information about FRAs when I need it
SMIS3 Social media helps me to keep updated on the latest news and events about FRAs
IT1 I believe that FRAs provide safe services Initial trust Kaabachi et al. (2019)
IT2 I believe that FRAs provide detailed information about their terms and conditions
IT3 I believe that FRAs provide accurate services
IT4 I believe that FRAs are trustworthy
BIU1 I intend to use FRAs in the near future Behavioural intention to use Oliveira et al. (2016), Venkatesh et al. (2012)
BIU2 I plan to use FRAs in the near future
BIU3 I will try to use FRAs in the near future
Decision style index
Item Operationalization Concept Source
RDS1 I prefer to gather all the necessary information before committing to a decision Rational items Hamilton et al. (2016)
RDS2 I thoroughly evaluate decision alternatives before making a final choice
RDS3 In decision-making, I take time to contemplate the pros/cons or risks/benefits of a situation
RDS4 Investigating the facts is an important part of my decision-making process
RDS5 I weigh a number of different factors when making decisions
IDS1 When making decisions, I rely mainly on my gut feelings Intuitive items
IDS2 My initial hunch about decisions is generally what I follow
IDS3 I make decisions based on intuition
IDS4 I rely on my first impressions when making decisions
IDS5 I weigh feelings more than analysis in making decisions

Appendix

Table A1

References

Abraham, F., Schmukler, S.L. and Josè, T. (2019), “Robo-advisors: investing through machines”, World Bank Chile Center and Malaysia Hub, available at: https://documents1.worldbank.org/curated/en/275041551196836758/pdf/Robo-Advisors-Investing-through-Machines.pdf (accessed 13 August 2021).

Akturan, U. and Tezcan, N. (2012), “Mobile banking adoption of the youth market”, Marketing Intelligence and Planning, Vol. 30 No. 4, pp. 444-459.

Alalwan, A., Dwivedi, Y., Rana, N. and Williams, M. (2016), “Consumer adoption of mobile banking in Jordan examining the role of usefulness, ease of use, perceived risk and self-efficacy”, Journal of Enterprise Information Management, Vol. 29 No. 1, pp. 118-139.

Al-Khalaf, E. and Choe, P. (2020), “Increasing customer trust towards mobile commerce in a multicultural society: a case of Qatar”, Journal of Internet Commerce, Vol. 19 No. 1, pp. 32-61.

Alkraiji, A. and Ameen, N. (2021), “The impact of service quality, trust and satisfaction on young citizen loyalty towards government e-services”, Information Technology and People, Vol. ahead-of-print No. ahead-of-print, doi: 10.1108/ITP-04-2020-0229.

Ameen, N., Hosany, S. and Tarhini, A. (2021), “Consumer interaction with cutting-edge technologies: implications for future research”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 120 No. February, pp. 1-7.

Amirtha, R., Sivakumar, V.J. and Hwang, Y. (2021), “Influence of perceived risk dimensions on e-shopping behavioural intention among women – a family life cycle stage perspective”, Journal of Theoretical and Applied Electronic Commerce Research, Vol. 16 No. 3, pp. 320-355.

Anagnostopoulos, I. (2018), “Fintech and regtech: impact on regulators and banks”, Journal of Economics and Business, Vol. 100 No. November-December, pp. 7-25.

Anderson, J.C. and Gerbing, D.W. (1988), “Structural equation weden a in practice: a review and recommended two-step approach”, Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 103 No. 3, pp. 411-423.

Andrikopoulos, P. and Vagenas-Nanos, E. (2017), “Special issue of the review of behavioral finance guest editors’ introduction”, Review of Behavioral Finance, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 102-104.

Ayton, P., Bernile, G., Bucciol, A. and Zarri, L. (2020), “The impact of life experiences on risk taking”, Journal of Economic Psychology, Vol. 79 No. April, pp. 1-4.

Baker, K. and Nofsinger, J. (2010), Behavioral Finance: Investors, Corporations, and Markets, John Wiley and Sons.

Bapat, D. (2020), “Antecedents to responsible financial management behavior among young adults: moderating role of financial risk tolerance”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 38 No. 5, pp. 1177-1194.

Bauer, R. (1960), “Consumer behaviour as risk-taking”, in Hancock, R. (Ed.), Dynamic Marketing for a Changing World, American Marketing Association, Chicago, IL, pp. 389-398.

Beketov, M., Lehmann, K. and Wittke, M. (2018), “Robo advisors: quantitative methods inside the robots”, Journal of Asset Management, Vol. 19 No. 6, pp. 363-370.

Berg, T., Burg, V., Gombović, A. and Puri, M. (2020), “On the rise of fintechs: credit scoring using digital footprints”, The Review of Financial Studies, Vol. 33 No. 7, pp. 2845-2897.

Bhatia, A., Chandani, A. and Chhateja, J. (2020), “Robo advisory and its potential in addressing the behavioral biases of investors – a qualitative study in Indian context”, Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, Vol. 25 No. March, pp. 1-9.

Brenner, L. and Meyll, T. (2020), “Robo-advisors: a substitute for human financial advice?”, Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, Vol. 25 No. March, pp. 1-8.

Chen, C.S. (2013), “Perceived risk, usage frequency of mobile banking services”, Managing Service Quality: An International Journal, Vol. 23 No. 5, pp. 410-436.

Cheng, X., Guo, F., Chen, J., Li, K., Zhang, Y. and Gao, P. (2019), “Exploring the trust influencing mechanism of robo-advisor service: a mixed method approach”, Sustainability, Vol. 11 No. 18, pp. 1-20.

Chiu, C.L., Chiu, J.L. and Mansumitrchai, S. (2016), “Privacy, security, infrastructure and cost issues in internet banking in the Philippines: initial trust formation”, International Journal of Financial Services Management, Vol. 8 No. 3, pp. 240-271.

Chong, L.L., Ong, H.B. and Tan, S.H. (2021), “Acceptability of mobile stock trading application: a study of young investors in Malaysia”, Technology in Society, Vol. 64 No. February, pp. 1-8.

Cosenza, M., Ciccarelli, M. and Nigro, G. (2019), “Decision-making styles, negative affectivity, and cognitive distortions in adolescent gambling”, Journal of Gambling Studies, Vol. 35 No. 2, pp. 517-531.

D’Acunto, F., Prabhala, N. and Rossi, A.G. (2019), “The promises and pitfalls of robo-advising”, The Review of Financial Studies, Vol. 32 No. 5, pp. 1983-2020.

DeBondt, W., Forbes, W., Hamalainen, P. and Gulnur Muradoglu, Y. (2010), “What can behavioural finance teach us about finance?”, Qualitative Research in Financial Markets, Vol. 2 No. 1, pp. 29-36.

Dharmesti, M., Dharmesti, T.R.S., Kuhne, S. and Thaichon, P. (2021), “Understanding online shopping behaviours and purchase intentions amongst millennials”, Young Consumers, Vol. 22 No. 1, pp. 152-167.

European Commission (2018), “Distribution systems of retail investment products across the European union”, available at: https://doi.org/10.2874/037900 (accessed 13 August 2021).

Featherman, M.S. and Pavlou, P.A. (2003), “Predicting e-services adoption: a perceived risk facets perspective”, International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, Vol. 59 No. 4, pp. 451-474.

Financial Industry Regulatory Authority (2016), “Investors in the United States 2016”, December, available at: www.usfinancialcapability.org/downloads/NFCS_2015_Inv_Survey_Full_Report.pdf (accessed 27 April 2021).

Fishbein, M. and Ajzen, I. (1975), “Formation of intentions”, Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research, pp. 288-334.

Florendo, J. and Estelami, H. (2019), “The role of cognitive style, gullibility, and demographics on the use of social media for financial decision making”, Journal of Financial Services Marketing, Vol. 24 Nos 1/2, pp. 1-10.

Fornell, C. and Larcker, D.F. (1981), “Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 18 No. 1, pp. 39-50.

Fulk, M., Grable, J.E., Watkins, K. and Kruger, M. (2018), “Who uses robo-advisory services, and who does not?”, Financial Services Review, Vol. 27 No. 2, pp. 173-188.

Gambetti, E. and Giusberti, F. (2019), “Personality, decision-making styles and investments”, Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, Vol. 80 No. June, pp. 14-24.

Gan, L.Y., Khan, M.T.I. and Liew, T.W. (2021), “Understanding consumer’s adoption of financial robo-advisors at the outbreak of the COVID-19 crisis in Malaysia”, Financial Planning Review, Vol. 4 No. 3, pp. 1-18, doi: 10.1002/cfp2.1127.

Goldstein, I., Jiang, W. and Karolyi, G.A. (2019), “To FinTech and beyond”, The Review of Financial Studies, Vol. 32 No. 5, pp. 1647-1661.

Gomber, P., Koch, J.-A. and Siering, M. (2017), “Digital finance and FinTech: current research and future research directions”, Journal of Business Economics, Vol. 87 No. 5, pp. 537-580.

Gudmunson, C.G., Ray, S.K. and Xiao, J.J. (2016), “Financial socialization”, in Xiao J.J. (Ed), Handbook of Consumer Finance Research, (pp. 61-72). Springer, Cham.

Hair, J., Risher, J.J., Sarstedt, M. and Ringle, C.M. (2019), “When to use and how to report the results of PLS-SEM”, European Business Review, Vol. 31 No. 1, pp. 2-24.

Hamilton, K., Shih, S.I. and Mohammed, S. (2016), “The development and validation of the rational and intuitive decision styles scale”, Journal of Personality Assessment, Vol. 98 No. 5, pp. 523-535.

Hamilton, K., Shih, S.I. and Mohammed, S. (2017), “The predictive validity of the decision styles scale: an evaluation across task types”, Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 119 No. December, pp. 333-340.

Henseler, J., Ringle, C.M. and Sarstedt, M. (2016), “Testing measurement invariance of composites using partial least squares”, International Marketing Review, Vol. 33 No. 3, pp. 405-431.

Hirshleifer, D. (2015), “Behavioral finance”, Annual Review of Financial Economics, Vol. 7 No. 1, pp. 133-159.

Hofstede, G. (2001), Culture’s Consequences: comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions, and Organizations across Nations, SAGE Publications, CA.

Jacoby, J. and Kaplan, L. (1972), “The components of perceived risk”, Third Annual Conference of the Association for Consumer Research, pp. 382-393.

Jung, D., Dorner, V., Glaser, F. and Morana, S. (2018b), “Robo-advisory: digitalization and automation of financial advisory”, Business and Information Systems Engineering, Vol. 60 No. 1, pp. 81-86.

Jung, D., Dorner, V., Weinhardt, C. and Pusmaz, H. (2018a), “Designing a robo-advisor for risk-averse, low-budget consumers”, Electronic Markets, Vol. 28 No. 3, pp. 367-380.

Jünger, M. and Mietzner, M. (2020), “Banking goes digital: the adoption of FinTech services by German households”, Finance Research Letters, Vol. 34, p. 101260.

Kaabachi, S., Ben Mrad, S. and O’Leary, B. (2019), “Consumer’s initial trust formation in IOB’s acceptance”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 37 No. 2, pp. 507-530.

Kahneman, D. (2011), Thinking, Fast and Slow, Farrar, Straus and Giroux, New York, NY.

Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1979), “Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk”, Econometrica, Vol. 47 No. 2, pp. 263-292.

Kinder, G. (2015), “Life planning and the robo-adviser role”, Journal of Financial Planning, Vol. 28 No. 6, pp. 34-35.

Koestner, M., Loos, B., Meyer, S., Hackethal, A., Loos, B. and Meyer, S. (2017), “Do individual investors learn from their mistakes?”, Journal of Business Economics, Vol. 87 No. 5, pp. 669-703.

Kostovetsky, L. (2016), “Whom do you trust?: investor-advisor relationships and mutual fund flows”, Review of Financial Studies, Vol. 29 No. 4, pp. 898-936.

Laroche, M., Habibi, M.R., Richard, M.O. and Sankaranarayanan, R. (2012), “The effects of social media based brand communities on brand community markers, value creation practices, brand trust and brand loyalty”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 28 No. 5, pp. 1755-1767.

Lee, M.C. (2009), “Factors influencing the adoption of internet banking: an integration of TAM and TPB with perceived risk and perceived benefit”, Electronic Commerce Research and Applications, Vol. 8 No. 3, pp. 130-141.

Lee, J.-M. and Kim, H.-J. (2020), “Determinants of adoption and continuance intentions toward internet-only banks”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 38 No. 4, pp. 843-865, doi: 10.1108/IJBM-07-2019-0269.

Lee, C.S. and Ma, L. (2012), “News sharing in social media: the effect of gratifications and prior experience”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 28 No. 2, pp. 331-339.

Litterscheidt, R. and Streich, D.J. (2020), “Financial education and digital asset management: what’s in the black box?”, Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, Vol. 87 No. August, pp. 1-12.

McKnight, D.H., Cummings, L.L. and Chervany, N.L. (1998), “Initial trust formation in new organizational relationships”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 23 No. 3, pp. 473-490.

Mitchell, V. (1999), “Consumer perceived risk: conceptualisations and models”, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 33 Nos 1/2, pp. 163-195.

Nourallah, M. (2022), “Mobile bank applications: antecedents and consequences of young bank customer loyalty”, International Journal of Management Practice, Vol. 15 No. 1, pp. 131-146, doi: 10.1504/IJMP.2021.10040611.

Nourallah, M. and Öhman, P. (2021), “Impact of advanced technologies on consumer finance and retail investment”, in Ho, R., Ng, A.H.H. and Nourallah, M. (Eds), Impact of Globalization and Advanced Technologies on Online Business Models, IGI Global, PA, pp. 1-15.

Ofori, K.S., Boakye, K.G., Addae, J.A., Ampong, G.O.A. and Adu, A.S.Y. (2018), “An empirical study on the adoption of consumer-to-consumer e-commerce: integrating the UTAUT model and the initial trust model”, Lecture Notes of the Institute for Computer Sciences, Social-Informatics and Telecommunications Engineering, LNICST, Vol. 250, pp. 281-292.

Oliveira, T., Thomas, M., Baptista, G. and Campos, F. (2016), “Mobile payment: understanding the determinants of customer adoption and intention to recommend the technology”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 61 No. August, pp. 404-414.

Otto, P.E. (2007), “Cognitive finance: behavioral strategies of spending, saving and investing”, Doctoral thesis, Department of Psychology, University College London, available at: https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/id/eprint/1444969/1/U592281.pdf

Pentina, I., Zhang, L. and Basmanova, O. (2013), “Antecedents and consequences of trust in a social media brand: a cross-cultural study of twitter”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 29 No. 4, pp. 1546-1555.

Rezaei, S., Shahijan, M.K., Amin, M. and Ismail, W.K.W. (2016), “Determinants of app stores continuance behavior: a PLS path modelling approach”, Journal of Internet Commerce, Vol. 15 No. 4, pp. 408-440.

Ringle, C.M., Wende, S. and Will, S. (2005), SmartPLS 2.0 (M3) Beta, Hamburg.

Rousseau, D.M., Sitkin, S.B., Burt, R.S. and Camerer, C. (1998), “Not so different after all: a cross-discipline view of trust”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 23 No. 3, pp. 393-404.

Scherer, B. (2017), “Algorithmic portfolio choice: lessons from panel survey data”, Financial Markets and Portfolio Management, Vol. 31 No. 1, pp. 49-67.

Schoorman, F., Mayer, R. and Davis, J. (2007), “An integrative model of organizational trust: past, present, and future”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 32 No. 2, pp. 344-354.

Seiler, V. and Fanenbruck, K.M. (2021), “Acceptance of digital investment solutions: the case of robo advisory in Germany”, Research in International Business and Finance, Vol. 58 No. December, pp. 1-14.

Shaheen, M., Zeba, F., Chatterjee, N. and Krishnankutty, R. (2020), “Engaging customers through credible and useful reviews: the role of online trust”, Young Consumers, Vol. 21 No. 2, pp. 137-153.

Simon, H.A. (1991), “Bounded rationality and organizational learning”, Organization Science, Vol. 2 No. 1, pp. 125-134.

Stålnacke, O. (2019), “Individual investors’ information use, subjective expectations, and portfolio risk and return”, The European Journal of Finance, Vol. 25 No. 15, pp. 1351-1376.

Statista (2019a), “Robo-advisors – Malaysia | statista market forecast”, www.Statista.Com/, available at: www.statista.com/outlook/337/122/robo-advisors/wedena#market-arpu (accessed 2 February 2020).

Statista (2019b), “Robo-advisors – Sweden | statista market forecast”, www.StatistaCom, available at: www.statista.com/outlook/337/154/robo-advisors/weden (accessed 2 February 2020).

Tam, C. and Oliveira, T. (2017), “Literature review of mobile banking and individual performance”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 35 No. 7, pp. 1042-1065.

Tan, D.S. and Tas, O. (2021), “Social media sentiment in international stock returns and trading activity”, Journal of Behavioral Finance, Vol. 22 No. 2, pp. 221-234.

Tandon, U., Kiran, R. and Sah, A.N. (2018), “The influence of website functionality, drivers and perceived risk on customer satisfaction in online shopping: an emerging economy case”, Information Systems and e-Business Management, Vol. 16 No. 1, pp. 57-91.

Thaler, R. (1993), Advances in Behavioural Finance, Russel Sage, New York, NY.

TIAA (2021), “2021 TIAA digital engagement survey”, available at: www.tiaa.org/public/pdf/digital_engagement_survey.pdf (accessed 10 August 2021).

Venkatesh, V., Thong, J.Y.L. and Xu, X. (2012), “Consumer acceptance and use of information technology: extending the unified theory of acceptance and use of technology”, MIS Quarterly: Management Information Systems, Vol. 36 No. 1, pp. 157-178.

Vlaev, I., Chater, N. and Stewart, N. (2009), “Dimensionality of risk perception: factors affecting consumer understanding and evaluation of financial risk”, Journal of Behavioral Finance, Vol. 10 No. 3, pp. 158-181.

Welch, I. (2022), “The wisdom of the Robinhood crowd”, The Journal of Finance, Vol. 77 No. 3, doi: 10.1111/jofi.13128.

Wu, M. and Gao, Q. (2021), “Understanding the acceptance of robo-advisors: towards a hierarchical model integrated product features and user perceptions”, in Gao, Q. and Zhou, J. (Eds), Human Aspects of IT for the Aged Population. Technology Design and Acceptance, Springer, pp. 262-277.

Yoshida, M., Gordon, B.S., Nakazawa, M., Shibuya, S. and Fujiwara, N. (2018), “Bridging the gap between social media and behavioral brand loyalty”, Electronic Commerce Research and Applications, Vol. 28 No. March – April, pp. 208-218.

Zeithaml, V.A., Berry, L.L. and Parasuraman, A. (1996), “The behavioral consequences of service quality”, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 60 No. 2, pp. 31-46.

Zhou, T. (2012), “Understanding users’ initial trust in mobile banking: an elaboration likelihood perspective”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 28 No. 4, pp. 1518-1525.

Zhu, X.S., Wolfson, M.A., Dalal, D.K. and Mathieu, J.E. (2021), “Team decision making: the dynamic effects of team decision style composition and performance via decision strategy”, Journal of Management, Vol. 47 No. 5, pp. 1281-1304.

Corresponding author

Mustafa Nourallah can be contacted at: mustafa.nourallah@miun.se

About the authors

Mustafa Nourallah is a Doctoral Student at the Centre for research on Economic Relations at Mid Sweden University, Sweden. His research focuses on FinTech (mobile banking service and Robo-financial advisor) and young retail investors and young bank customers.

Peter Öhman (PhD) is a Professor of Business Administration at Mid Sweden University and the Centre for Research on Economic Relations. His research focuses on accounting, auditing and banking.

Muslim Amin (PhD) is a Professor of Services Management at Sunway University Business School, Sunway University, Malaysia. His research focuses on financial services marketing, services management and hospitality management.

Related articles