Silence is golden: the role of team coordination in health operations

Alessandro Stefanini (University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy)
Davide Aloini (University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy)
Peter Gloor (Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA)

International Journal of Operations & Production Management

ISSN: 0144-3577

Article publication date: 14 May 2020

Issue publication date: 30 November 2020

1747

Abstract

Purpose

This study investigates the relationships between team dynamics and performance in healthcare operations. Specifically, it explores, through wearable sensors, how team coordination mechanisms can influence the likelihood of surgical glitches during routine surgery.

Design/methodology/approach

Breast surgeries of a large Italian university hospital were monitored using Sociometric Badges – wearable sensors developed at MIT Media Lab – for collecting objective and systematic measures of individual and group behaviors in real time. Data retrieved were used to analyze team coordination mechanisms, as it evolved in the real settings, and finally to test the research hypotheses.

Findings

Findings highlight that a relevant portion of glitches in routine surgery is caused by improper team coordination practices. In particular, results show that the likelihood of glitches decreases when practitioners adopt implicit coordination mechanisms rather than explicit ones. In addition, team cohesion appears to be positively related with the surgical performance.

Originality/value

For the first time, direct, objective and real time measurements of team behaviors have enabled an in-depth evaluation of the team coordination mechanisms in surgery and the impact on surgical glitches. From a methodological perspective, this research also represents an early attempt to investigate coordination behaviors in dynamic and complex operating environments using wearable sensor tools.

Keywords

Citation

Stefanini, A., Aloini, D. and Gloor, P. (2020), "Silence is golden: the role of team coordination in health operations", International Journal of Operations & Production Management, Vol. 40 No. 9, pp. 1421-1447. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJOPM-12-2019-0792

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2020, Alessandro Stefanini, Davide Aloini and Peter Gloor

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


1. Introduction

Healthcare operations have become increasingly complex and interdisciplinary. The use and role of teams has thus grown significantly in healthcare organizations making teamwork an essential determinant of effective care delivery (Valentine et al., 2015). Healthcare teams, especially in dynamic domains such as operating rooms, emergency medicine and intensive care, are typically multidisciplinary, work under quickly-changing conditions, and stay together for short periods with a dynamically fluctuating team membership (Bamford and Griffin, 2008; Denton, 2013; Vashdi et al., 2013). These characteristics make healthcare teams very interesting but also hard to study (Shah and Breazeal, 2010; Wheelock et al., 2015; Chang et al., 2017).

The literature on operations management highlights the high impact that teamwork has on team performance and especially on the potential operational failures. Team dynamics are defined as behavioral relationships between members and the influential interpersonal processes that occur in teams over time (Levi, 2015; Forsyth, 2018), and seem to play a central role in healthcare (Manser, 2009; Broekhuis and van Donk, 2011; Berry et al., 2016). This position is supported by many authors in various areas of healthcare: surgery (e.g. Mazzocco et al., 2009; Siu et al., 2016); intensive care (e.g. Dietz et al., 2014); mental health (e.g. Deacon and Cleary, 2013); neonatal resuscitation (e.g. Williams et al., 2010); and across entire hospital systems (e.g. Berry et al., 2016). Consequently, researchers and health managers have investigated team dynamics in various healthcare settings to understand the determinants of performance and to define strategies for improving teamwork and team operational performance (Manser, 2009).

In this context, surgery has particularly attracted attention above all as a result of its impact on hospital budgets and patient care and safety (Jebali et al., 2006; Gupta, 2007; Venkataraman et al., 2018). The operating room is one of the most complex and challenging work environments in healthcare, where teamwork failures and human errors have been shown to cause the greatest amount of unintentional harm in a hospital (Wheelock et al., 2015; Sun et al., 2018; Rubbio et al., 2019).

Team dynamics exert a great influence on surgical performance (Healey et al., 2004; Siu et al., 2016; Venkataraman et al., 2018). Specifically, team coordination seems to play a key role in the management of dynamic work teams (Mathieu et al., 2008; Rico et al., 2008; Gorman, 2014), such as surgical teams. Team coordination, i.e. the behaviors that team members adopt to coordinate themselves during operational tasks, takes place through explicit and implicit coordination mechanisms and patterns (see Section 2.1. for a definition of these mechanisms). The balance between these two coordination mechanisms, which occur simultaneously in surgical teams, has a profound influence on team performance (Malone and Crowston, 1994; Crawford and LePine, 2013).

Although implicit coordination appears to increase team effectiveness, the best equilibrium between explicit and implicit coordination depends on the specific features of the operational tasks and the related operational context (Broekhuis and van Donk, 2011; Gorman, 2014; Butchibabu et al., 2016). Accordingly, the best coordination strategy depends on how frequently a surgical procedure is carried out and the levels of uncertainty involved. Consequently, surgical teams should have different coordination behaviors with respect to routine and non-routine surgeries (Manser, 2009; Bogdanovic et al., 2015).

Although routine surgeries are by far the most frequent in hospitals and overall have the greatest operational impact (e.g. Abbott et al., 2017; Steiner et al., 2017), past research has tended to focus on the less routine interventions since they are likely to be more interesting from a medical perspective (Seelandt et al., 2014; Siu et al., 2016; Gjeraa et al., 2017).

We thus believe that an in-depth exploration of team coordination practices in routine surgery is crucial for helping healthcare organizations and researcher to improve health performance and patient safety. Evidence of direct relationships between team coordination practices and surgical outcomes are, in fact, scarce and not conclusive (Mazzocco et al., 2009; Sun et al., 2018).

To fill this gap, we therefore address the following Research Question:

RQ.

How does team coordination tend to influence operational performance in routine surgeries?

Specifically, we explore the effects of different team coordination mechanisms, both explicit and implicit, on surgical outcomes through the evidence provided by a case study within the Breast Surgery Unit of an Italian university hospital. In addition to team coordination mechanisms, Team Cohesion was also taken in account in this investigation. Team cohesion, in fact, seems to play a key role in the dynamics and effectiveness of team coordination in surgery (Burtscher and Manser, 2012; Gorman, 2014; Sacks et al., 2015).

This study exploits wearable sensors in terms of their ability to obtain automatic and objective measurements of individual and group behaviors. In fact, one of the main reasons for the lack of strong evidence in this context appears to be related to the difficulty in directly measuring coordination behaviors during real surgical procedures (Rosen et al., 2014; Sun et al., 2018).

The contribution of this work is in its assessment of the influence of team coordination on surgical performance in routine surgery, which are the most relevant from an operational viewpoint (Abbott et al., 2017; Steiner et al., 2017). The uniqueness of this study comes from our in-depth exploration of team coordination behaviors. We recorded in real-time many different aspects of practitioner behaviors during surgical procedures (e.g. body movement, speaking interactions, and proximity). To the best of our knowledge, this is the first time that such detailed individual and collective coordination behaviors have been evaluated while medical teams are carrying out surgery and then subsequently related to surgical outcomes.

We believe that our results will help (1) practitioners and health managers by highlighting sub-optimal coordination practices that seem to affect operational failures in routine surgery, and (2) health providers in training surgical staff and in planning surgical activities.

Our study also contributes to the operations management methodology. For the first time wearable sensors have been exploited for investigating relationships between team coordination behaviors and process performance in such a dynamic context (Rosen et al., 2014; Chaffin et al., 2017). Accordingly, this research also provides a first answer to the question of “how” to quantitatively investigate team dynamics and behavioral factors in healthcare operations and in other complex business environments (Croson et al., 2013; Brocklesby, 2016).

The paper is structured as follows: Section 2 overviews related studies on team coordination and develops the research hypotheses. Section 3 presents the research method and describes the measurements. Section 4 reports the findings. Section 5 discusses the results and outlines the main contributions. Finally, Section 6 discusses the limitations of the method and suggests s directions for future research.

2. Theoretical background and hypotheses

2.1 The role of implicit - explicit coordination mechanisms and cohesion in teamwork

Coordination of work teams, defined as the actions of managing tasks performed by separate actors to achieve a common goal, is an important issue for operations management, particularly given the increasing number of business activities organized in teams (Malone and Crowston, 1994; Marks et al., 2001; Mathieu et al., 2008; Easton and Rosenzweig, 2015; Ben-Menahem et al., 2016). Coordination enables team efforts to be organized and guided to predefined targets through various mechanisms, such as verbal communication, task assignment, and written feedbacks (Brannick and Prince, 1997). Past studies have shown that an appropriate level of coordination is very important for teams across all sectors and that poor team coordination has a negative impact on outcomes (Faraj and Sproull, 2000; Mathieu et al., 2008).

Team coordination includes explicit and implicit coordination, which occur simultaneously during team operations. Explicit coordination refers to visible and external coordination mechanisms and patterns, such as mutual communication, direct monitoring, and team rules, which team members use intentionally to manage their multiple interdependencies (Malone and Crowston, 1994; Espinosa et al., 2004; Ben-Menahem et al., 2016). In work teams dedicated to a specific and pre-defined task, explicit coordination corresponds mostly to the transfer of information and resources in response to a direct request, which usually happens through verbal communication (Entin and Serfaty, 1999; Butchibabu et al., 2016).

On the contrary, implicit coordination patterns operate imperceptibly and, thus, are more difficult to observe. Implicit coordination occurs when team members anticipate the actions and needs of their colleagues and dynamically adjust their own behavior accordingly, without the need for direct communication (Espinosa et al., 2004; Rico et al., 2008).

Given the requests for information amongst teammates, explicit coordination implies a higher communication overhead than implicit coordination, which implies anticipatory information sharing behaviors (Entin and Serfaty, 1999; Shah and Breazeal, 2010; Butchibabu et al., 2016). Some studies have theorized (e.g. Rico et al., 2008; Gorman, 2014) and have empirically shown (e.g. Butchibabu et al., 2016) that implicit coordination, which frees up individuals' mental resources and contributes to harmonizing team activities, increases work team effectiveness and efficiency, while excessive explicit coordination behaviors may negatively affect team outcomes. Therefore, the right balance needs to be struck between explicit and implicit coordination, and this depends on the characteristics of the task involved.

In line with contingency theory, the appropriateness of team coordination mechanisms might depend on the specific operational context, and in particular on the level of task uncertainty and interdependence (Malone and Crowston, 1994; Donaldson, 2001; Okhuysen and Bechky, 2009; Broekhuis and van Donk, 2011; Crawford and LePine, 2013).

For instance, in highly interdependent tasks, in order to complete a task successfully team members should coordinate their actions and share their information continuously. In such cases, explicit coordination might lead team members to spend too much time and energy on coordination rather than on performing their jobs. Implicit coordination is thus likely to have a greater positive effect on team performance when teams perform highly interdependent tasks (Espinosa et al., 2004; Rico et al., 2008).

Regarding task uncertainty, when task routineness is high – and thus there is low task uncertainty – implicit coordination seems to improve team performance. In contrast, when task routineness is low (high uncertainty), the exclusive use of implicit coordination might have a negative effect on team performance (Espinosa et al., 2004; Rico et al., 2008; Broekhuis and van Donk, 2011; Riethmüller et al., 2012). In fact, when tasks are ill defined, uncertain, and characterized by frequently-changing requirements, explicit coordination is needed to synchronize team members throughout unique and non-repetitive actions.

Past studies also show that in many situations where explicit coordination is avoidable, as occurs in routine tasks, a lower level of explicit communication leads to higher team performance (e.g. Manser, 2009; Gorman, 2014; Butchibabu et al., 2016). This is justified by the decrease in the coordination overheads. Explicit coordination involves the mental efforts of team members which might increase the level of distraction during operational activities. This appears to be confirmed by empirical research across different business settings (Shah and Breazeal, 2010; Wheelock et al., 2015; Chang et al., 2017).

The effectiveness of coordination in work teams also seems to be influenced by Team Cohesion, i.e. the degree of shared ties and member integration that drive team members to work together, to commit to other members, and to accomplish the purposes of the team (Zaccaro et al., 2001; Mathieu et al., 2015; Salas et al., 2015). Team cohesion mostly depends on the existence of sound shared mental models and on the development of a collaborative and constructive working environment, where everyone's contribution is appreciated and individual team members can freely share their ideas without being criticized (Kozlowski and Chao, 2012). Indeed, shared team mental models are defined as common knowledge structures held by members of a team that enable them to have shared expectations and explanations regarding the task and, thus to coordinate their actions and adapt their behaviors based on the objectives and behaviors of other members (Cannon-Bowers et al., 1993; DeChurch and Mesmer-Magnus, 2010; Healey et al., 2015). In this way, shared knowledge and a positive and collaborative environment enable team members to anticipate and predict each other's behaviors, and to back each other up during the team's activities (MacMillan et al., 2004; Smith-Jentsch et al., 2009; Kozlowski and Chao, 2012; Vashdi et al., 2013; Gorman, 2014).

Past studies have shown that cohesive teams communicate more efficiently and use implicit coordination mechanisms more intensively, enhancing team coordination and team effectiveness (Stout et al., 1999; Rico et al., 2008; Fisher et al., 2012; Gorman, 2014). Specifically, it appears that the impact of team cohesion on team performance is very important in many different team configurations and across sectors (Mathieu et al., 2000, 2008; Burtscher and Manser, 2012; DeChurch and Mesmer-Magnus, 2010), though the influence on performance seems to be greater where a high level of implicit coordination is required (Rico et al., 2008; Gorman, 2014).

2.2 Empirical research on team coordination in healthcare operations

Teamwork is increasingly assuming a crucial role in healthcare operations following the increase in care activities carried out by teams (Mitchell and Golden, 2012; Valentine et al., 2015). Recognizing the relevance of team dynamics for patient care and safety, a growing number of studies have focused on the relationships between team coordination and related outcomes (Burtscher and Manser, 2012; Rosen et al., 2018), so as to be able to provide indications for improving healthcare results in terms of operational effectiveness and efficiency (Sun et al., 2018).

Due to the huge variety of healthcare activities, team coordination and team-building have been studied both in acute settings, such as emergency or surgery units, and in non-acute settings, such as primary care or rehabilitation clinics (Boyer and Pronovost, 2010; Miller et al., 2018). However, it is on acute care settings that researchers have mostly focused so far. This is likely due to the high potential impact of coordination and teamwork on patient safety and care effectiveness for such short-term teams (Bamford and Griffin, 2008; Manser, 2009; Mazzocco et al., 2009; Johnston et al., 2019). In fact, without proper team coordination, wrong decisions can be made which affect team performance or even fatally compromise the patient safety (Siu et al., 2016; Thomas et al., 2018).

Although the issue of team coordination in healthcare has been explored, some methodological concerns still remain in investigating relationships between team dynamics and healthcare performance (Schmutz and Manser, 2013; Denton, 2013; Tiferes et al., 2015; Berry et al., 2016; Donohue et al., 2018). The main challenge, not only in healthcare, is how to systematically and objectively measure team behaviors in such a dynamic environment (Kim et al., 2012; Schmutz and Manser, 2013; Chaffin et al., 2017).

Past research has essentially exploited two approaches for measuring team behavior: self-reporting and observational methods (Healey et al., 2004; Manser, 2009; Rosen et al., 2014; Carayon et al., 2015; Senot et al., 2016). With self-reporting methods, team members report and/or rate their perceptions about team behavior and coordination mechanisms through interviews, questionnaires, focus groups, and surveys. These methods are subjected to “self-reporting bias” and have other possible limitations: data collection is not in real time but performed in batches and discontinuously (e.g. at the end of health treatment), which involves the problem of “memory effects” by participants; the number of variables kept under control/analysis must be limited; the qualitative measurement and evaluation of human behaviors usually suffers from subjectivity, with different scales of perception for each participant (Lepine et al., 2008; Kuula and Putkiranta, 2012; Schmutz and Manser, 2013; Rosen et al., 2014; Chaffin et al., 2017).

Conversely, observational methods, such as direct observation and video analysis, may be able to overcome the “self-reporting bias” due to memory effects and the subjectivity of participants (Schmutz and Manser, 2013; Morgan et al., 2015; Ellway, 2016). Nevertheless, observational methods might still be exposed to the subjectivity of the observers; they are extremely time-consuming, due to the long period of observation, training, and monitoring over time; and the influence of the observers on the observed system might alter the results (Barley, 1990; Leonard-Barton, 1990; Kim et al., 2012; Rosen et al., 2014; Chaffin et al., 2017). Furthermore, the observational methods can be affected by privacy concerns associated with the presence of observers or recording cameras in care settings.

Wearable sensors and similar technologies offer a novel means to measure and appraise teamwork through novel data-driven methodologies. Providing automatic and objective measurements of individual and group behaviors, these tools can efficiently collect a large amount of data in real time, thus increasing the number of observable behavioral variables (Kim et al., 2012; Rosen et al., 2014; Chaffin et al., 2017). Wearable sensors, possibly integrated with self-reporting and observational methods, might mitigate the limitations of the traditional methods used to study teamwork in healthcare and in other dynamic environments (Kim et al., 2012; Rosen et al., 2014; Chaffin et al., 2017).

2.3 Hypotheses development

Team coordination and effective communication play a key role in for providing high quality and safe patient care during surgery (Broekhuis and van Donk, 2011; Tiferes et al., 2015; Singer et al., 2016). Accordingly, a growing number of empirical studies have shown that errors and adverse events during surgery may be due to failures in team coordination and non-technical issues, rather than a lack of technical expertise (Healey et al., 2004; Yule et al., 2006; Schmutz and Manser, 2013; Siu et al., 2016; Gjeraa et al., 2017; Sun et al., 2018; Rubbio et al., 2019).

A communication and coordination strategy should thus be adapted to the specific type of surgery (Crawford and LePine, 2013; Vashdi et al., 2013). In particular, in line with what was outlined in Section 2.1, the appropriate balance between explicit and implicit coordination strategies depends on the particular characteristics of the surgery performed (Vashdi et al., 2013; Bogdanovic et al., 2015).

Specifically, the degree of repetition and uncertainty of surgical procedures mean that they can be classified as routine and non-routine surgery, and thus coordination best practices can be identified (Manser, 2009; Bogdanovic et al., 2015). Consequently, surgical teams should adapt their behaviors based on the task routineness of the specific surgery.

With regard to non-routine surgery, the exclusive use of implicit coordination seems to have a negative effect on surgical performance, because explicit coordination is more effective in synchronizing team members during moments of high uncertainty (Rico et al., 2008; Manser, 2009; Riethmüller et al., 2012).

Therefore, the amount of explicit coordination behavior, above all accomplished through verbal communication, should increase when the surgical procedures are non-standard and the level of task uncertainty is high (Tschan et al., 2015; Sun et al., 2018). In surgeries where the tasks are not well-defined at the beginning and change based on patient conditions/needs, operating teams should execute specific procedures defined during the surgery and, thus, they require explicit coordination to synchronize team members (Riethmüller et al., 2012; Bogdanovic et al., 2015). This does not mean that implicit coordination is irrelevant, but the importance of explicit coordination and unambiguous communication arises.

On the contrary, research seems to confirm that for routine surgeries, a lower level of explicit communication brings about higher team performance, while a high level of implicit coordination has a positive effect on performance (Broekhuis and van Donk, 2011; Wheelock et al., 2015; Butchibabu et al., 2016). When the standardization of a surgical procedure is high, a larger proportion of implicit coordination means that there can be a greater focus on the surgical task, since it frees up an individual's mental resources and there can thus be a more effective and efficient team communication (Manser, 2009; Gorman, 2014).

Intense discussions/talks in routine surgeries thus tend to be associated with a high probability of poorer surgical performance and increased risks to patient safety (Seelandt et al., 2014; Tschan et al., 2015; Wheelock et al., 2015; Singer et al., 2016). The importance of remaining vigilant throughout a case is also key, as is maintaining a highly efficient team operating in a silent environment, especially when the surgical procedure presents a high level of routineness (Wheelock et al., 2015; Tschan et al., 2015; Keller et al., 2016).

However, empirical evidence regarding the most appropriate coordination strategies during routine surgery still remains scarce and not definitive (Mazzocco et al., 2009; Sun et al., 2018). For this reason and since routine interventions are by far the most frequent in hospitals (e.g. Abbott et al., 2017; Steiner et al., 2017), this work focuses on the study of team coordination dynamics in such a context.

We thus focus on routine surgery, and we hypothesize a negative effect of explicit coordination, which implies higher communication overheads and reduced focus on surgical task, and a positive effect of implicit coordination.

Thus, our first hypothesis is:

H1.

The lower the explicit coordination of the surgical team through verbal interaction, the higher the team's performance

The dynamics and effectiveness of team coordination in surgeries also depend on Team Cohesion, recognizing, in particular, the important role of shared mental models (Cannon-Bowers et al., 1993; Mathieu et al., 2000; McComb and Simpson, 2014) and of a collaborative environment for providing high quality and safe patient care (Catchpole et al., 2008; Mazzocco et al., 2009; Ancarani et al., 2011; Kurmann et al., 2014; Siu et al., 2016). Indeed, members of cohesive teams working in a collaborative and trusting environment experience fewer conflicts and discussions, tend to anticipate and support each other, and are able to increase their performance thanks to reduced stress levels (Arora et al., 2010; Wheelock et al., 2015; Singer et al., 2016). In this way, team cohesion enhances implicit coordination and harmonizes team behaviors, thus improving their ability to cope with surgical problems, and to communicate more efficiently (Burtscher and Manser, 2012; Gorman, 2014; Sacks et al., 2015; Gjeraa et al., 2017). This in turn indirectly improves the quality and safety of surgical procedures.

The positive effects of team cohesion on team dynamics and on final surgical outcomes have been highlighted in several surgery contexts, irrespectively of the routine nature of the activities involved (Rico et al., 2008; Manser, 2009; Sacks et al., 2015). For instance, Gjeraa et al. (2017) found that team cohesion can increase patient safety during non-routine surgery, as shown by the case study in video-assisted thoracoscopic surgery. Catchpole et al. (2008) highlighted that also in routine surgeries, such as low-risk orthopedic operations, team building and understanding mutual needs decrease the occurrence of surgical problems. Nevertheless, the positive effect of team cohesion appears to be greater in contexts that need a high level of implicit coordination (Rico et al., 2008; Manser, 2009; Gorman, 2014; Bogdanovic et al., 2015).

In line with past evidence on team cohesion we hypothesize:

H2.

The higher the level of team cohesion, the higher the performance of the surgical team

3. Method

Given the lack of in-depth empirical research on the coordination behaviors of surgery teams and the novelty of our measurement approach, we conducted an exploratory case study (Yin, 2017) in the Breast Surgery Unit of a large university hospital.

The research design exploited wearable sensors (i.e. Sociometric Badges) to retrieve data regarding team coordination dynamically and in real time in real settings, as well as to investigate our two hypotheses (Fernandes et al., 2018).

Section 3.1 introduces the Sociometric Badges. Section 3.2 outlines how we collected the data, and Section 3.3 describes the measurements exploited in the research. Finally, the data analysis and the related results are reported in Section 4.

3.1 Sociometric Badges

A key challenge when studying team dynamics in the highly dynamic domains of healthcare, such as operating rooms, intensive care, and emergency medicine, is how to measure individual and group behaviors (i.e. the data collection phase) which are commonly assessed through interviews, direct observations, questionnaires, and reports (Manser, 2009; Carayon et al., 2015). Previous studies have often been influenced by subjectivity and memory effects, or have been affected by the influence of the observers on the system (Barley, 1990; Leonard-Barton, 1990; Kim et al., 2012; Chaffin et al., 2017).

In this study, we used for the first time direct and quantitative measurements provided by Sociometric Badges in order to investigate behaviors of surgical team members. Sociometric Badges are wearable sensors developed by the MIT Media Lab. They catch the “honest signals” inherently related to human behaviors and group interactions (Pentland, 2008). They can automatically and directly measure individual and group dynamics in a quantitative way, exploiting four different sensors: accelerometers, microphones, Bluetooth, and IRDA (Olguín et al., 2009a). In so doing, Sociometric Badges offer a systematic way to quantitatively investigate the individual and collective patterns of behavior and overcoming the traditional limitations.

Since privacy is one of the main concerns of any social science study, Sociometric Badges also guarantee compliance with privacy laws by not recording the content of the conversation and not permitting the identification of the speaker by analyzing the sociometric data - this is achieved through the sampling of audio signals which prevent the reconstruction of the raw speech (Olguín et al., 2009a; Kim et al., 2012).

Sociometric Badges are also less intrusive than a human observer, thus limiting social distortions of the data, and they can capture more nuances of behavior (Olguín et al., 2009a; Rosen et al., 2014). Sociometric Badges have proved suitable for monitoring individual and group behaviors during operational activities (Kim et al., 2012), including healthcare (Olguín et al., 2009b; Bucuvalas et al., 2014; Yu et al., 2016).

3.2 Data collection

Our case study concerns quadrantectomy and mastectomy related to cancer problems, in the Breast Unit of an Italian university hospital. These surgeries present a high level of task routineness and low uncertainty (e.g. McLaughlin, 2013), with the procedures being well defined beforehand.

The surgical team investigated in our study is made up of: two surgeons, one scrub nurse, and one assistant nurse. Frequently there are also one or two additional surgeons or nurses. In general, the analyzed team was made up of a minimum of 4 to a maximum of 7 team members.

A preliminary investigation of the surgical context, before the data collection phase, was conducted through interviews with all the practitioners involved and the direct observation of some surgeries. The aim was to identify all the features related to breast surgery, such as the task allocation, interactions between practitioners, and the practitioners' physical distribution in the operating room. This information was used to define the main research settings (e.g. the sociometric variables to be considered) and to allow Sociometric Badges to be fine-tuned (e.g. Kayhan et al., 2018).

During the data collection phase, we collected the individual and group behaviors of team members for each investigated surgery exclusively using the Sociometric Badges (an improved version of the original one) to quantitatively assess the surgical team coordination behaviors. Each team member wore the Sociometric Badge for the entire duration of the surgery. Sensors allowed the collection of behavior data thought the accelerometer, microphones, and Bluetooth data.

In addition to the sociometric data, we collected the main features of the surgeries (e.g. the duration of surgery, the practitioners' experience) and patients (e.g. age). We also asked team members to fill out a “team cohesion” survey regarding their perception of the level of cohesion in each different team they participated in. By examining the surgical register, we also investigated operating room performance in terms of surgical glitches occurring in each surgical procedure.

Table 1 summarizes all the data collected with their respective sources and the roles assumed in the analysis.

All the measurements adopted in the study are described Section 3.3. They are subdivided into dependent variables, independent variables, and control variables, based on their role in the statistical models.

Data were gathered over a period of four months. Specifically, we collected the data of about 90 surgical operations (after data cleaning and preprocessing, 82 were considered as useable) with 362 individual recordings for a total of about 470 monitoring hours. All observed surgeries were chosen randomly.

To respect patient anonymity and privacy, no personal patient data or vital parameters were collected except for their general characteristics (e.g. age). In addition, Sociometric Badges do not record the content of the conversation and do not permit the identification of the speaker (see Section 3.1). This guaranteed patients and practitioners privacy. To fully comply with the privacy laws and principles of “research ethics,” we followed the recommendations by Stopczynski et al. (2014) for sensor-driven human data collection. All participants read an information sheet on the tools exploited and signed an informed consent form for the research.

3.3 Measurements

3.3.1 Dependent variable

Each surgical performance was evaluated through the occurrence of surgical glitches. In fact, providing a safe and effective operation is the main goal of surgery and a surgical glitch may impact strongly on patient health and hospital expenditure.

We define the dependent variable Glitches ON/OFF as a binary variable to differentiate between the operations “with” and “without” surgical complications. Glitches ON/OFF takes the value of 1 if one or more surgical glitches affect the surgery, otherwise 0. In this study, we considered a surgical glitch as any medical/procedural problem that might affect the patient care or their length of stay in the OR, for example small bleedings, incorrect counting of gauzes, imperfect stitching, and defects in sampling for cancer tests. The binary classification of surgeries into two classes (with/without glitches) was decided following the suggestions of surgeons and unit managers. Indeed, a numerical count of surgical complications is very often questionable when multiple complications happen (possible “domino effect”).

3.3.2 Independent variables

We defined a series of independent variables for this study by exploiting the sensors embedded in the Sociometric Badges and the manufacturer's software. The software was used for data processing and for the analysis of the sensor data series, as well as for extracting the features and the characterization of collaboration behaviors for each work team by appropriately combining the series of individual data.

To assess the level of cohesion in a team, our independent variables were “team cohesion” and “team cohesion variance,” which were evaluated through a dedicated survey.

We divided the independent variables into five groups (Table 2), based on the sensors used and the type of measurements: Movement, Speech, Interaction, Group Flow, and Perception (measured by survey). Table 3 reports the complete list of measures, grouped by class.

3.3.3 Control variables

To exclude potential confounding effects derived from the specifics of the surgery, eight control variables were employed:

Operating team – The size of the surgical team (see Section 3.2), i.e. the number of people with the Sociometric Badges.

Type of breast surgery – This variable assumes two values: 0 for quadrantectomy and 1 for mastectomy.

Patient age – The age of the patient involved in the surgery, as this can affect the risk of surgical complications.

Surgery duration – The duration of the surgery in minutes. We did not include the preparation and post-operation activities, just the length of the actual operation.

People inside OR – The number of people inside the operating room (OR). In addition to the operating team, there were other people present who did not really intervene in the surgical activities, such as trainee nurses, anesthetists, external doctors, and other observers.

Practitioners' experience – The average surgery experience of operating team members expressed in years. In fact, the level of experience of practitioners can affect their behavior during the surgery.

Practitioners' experience variance – The variance of surgery experience of operating team members. Not only the average level of experience can characterize the surgery, but also highly different experience levels can influence individual and team behaviors (e.g. teaching behaviors of senior surgeons).

Team familiarity – The average seniority (years of service) in the unit of operating team members, as inter-personal experiences can influence individual and team behaviors.

4. Results

After excluding any surgical operations with incorrect registrations or missing values, “Pearson's correlation” (Section 4.1) and logistic regression (Section 4.2) were performed using the data collected during the breast surgeries to assess the proposed hypotheses. Practitioners' interviews, focus groups, and a few direct observations of breast surgeries were then exploited to interpret the results, to verify the absence of potential bias in the research, and to draw additional implications from a managerial viewpoint.

4.1 Correlation analysis

To gain some preliminary insights from the data, we performed Pearson's correlation between the independent variables, the dependent variable, and the control variables using SPSS®. Correlation analysis enabled us to explore the relationships between the behavioral variables and the dependent variable (i.e. Glitches ON/OFF); between different behavioral variables; and between the control variables and the dependent variable. Table A1 shows the results obtained. The findings achieved from this analysis allowed to better guide the subsequent phase of regression analysis, highlighting the strongest relationships and the presence of potential statistical criticalities (e.g. relationships between control variables and dependent variables).

Three statistically significant correlations between the dependent variable (Glitches ON/OFF) and the independent variables were discovered. The strongest (R = −0.421) and most significant (p = 0.000) correlation of Glitches ON/OFF was with Silence. The second significant correlation of the dependent variable was with Turn-Taking (R = 0.289), while the third was with Team Cohesion (R = −0.245).

Similarly, we considered the correlations between the control variables and the dependent variable to exclude any potential effects of the control variables. Operating team, Type of surgery, Patient age, Surgery duration, People inside OR, Practitioners' experience, Practitioners' experience variance, and Team familiarity were not significantly correlated with Glitches ON/OFF. This seems to confirm the absence of relationships between dependent and control variables. However, the influence of the control variables and of the collinearity between independent variables was investigated and is reported in the next section.

4.2 Regression analysis

To measure the potential effect of independent variables on the dependent variable, we adopted logistic regression, since Glitches (ON/OFF) is binary (Hosmer and Lemeshow, 2004).

Table 4 shows the logistic regression model obtained. The model revealed that Silence, Team Cohesion, and Mirroring explain a significant part of the variance in the occurrence of glitches. The model (Table 4) is strongly significant (p = 0.000) and explains 50.2% of the variance as reported by the Nagelkerke R2 (Nagelkerke, 1991). All the variables included are statistically significant (column “Sig.”). In addition, the Hosmer and Lemeshow test also confirms the significance of the model (Hosmer and Lemeshow, 2004). To counteract the potential problem of multicollinearity between the independent variables, we quantified the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF). The very low values obtained, which were way below the threshold value of 3 (O'brien, 2007), seem to rule out any multicollinearity among the independent variables.

Turn-Taking, which was correlated with the dependent variable, was excluded from the model due to multicollinearity. When included, it was not significant and presented a high VIF value.

The logistic model (Table 4) highlights the positive effect of Silence on performance, i.e. a higher value of Silence implies a lower likelihood of experiencing a surgical glitch. Mirroring, included with the negative sign, also has a positive effect on performance because it seems to reduce the chance of glitches during surgery. Considering Mirroring as a proxy for assessing the level of implicit coordination (see Section 3.3.2, Table 3) during surgery, the regression model suggests that implicit coordination behaviors positively affect team performance. Finally, Team Cohesion appears to positively affect surgical performance, as shown by the negative sign in the model.

To rule out the potentially key influence of glitches on the dynamics of the surgery (“reverse causality”) and confirm the results obtained, the trends during surgery of Silence and Mirroring were analyzed. This evaluation proved that both Silence and Mirroring are quite steady during surgeries and, above all, they do not vary substantially during (or after) a glitch, thus ruling out reverse causality problems.

The model supports hypothesis H1, by confirming that a high level of explicit coordination (high level of verbal interaction) negatively affects the outcome of routine surgeries, while implicit coordination (high level of mirroring) appears to favor it. Team members coordinating continuously through explicit verbal interactions seem to lose focus on the ongoing tasks and the probability of glitches typically increases. On the contrary, when they coordinate implicitly, they remain more focused on the job and the chance of glitches tends to decrease. This result is aligned with the insights gained from informal interviews that we had with practitioners and direct observations carried out to explain the findings obtained.

The model also supports hypothesis H2, showing the positive influence of the team cohesion on the surgical outcome through the decreased likelihood of glitches. Team cohesion seems to increase the supportive behaviors by team members, team communication efficacy, and practitioners' engagement. Thus, it positively affects surgical performance.

To further confirm the validity of our findings, we tested the models by evaluating the effect of the control variables. In the first test, we added the control variables, individually and together, to the regression model obtained with Silence, Team Cohesion, and Mirroring. For each model, the resulting Nagelkerke R2 did not change considerably and the control variables inserted were non-significant, while Silence, Team Cohesion, and Mirroring still remained significant. Table A2 reports the model obtained including the eight control variables.

In a second test, we built regression models exclusively with the control variables. Again, no model including the control variables was statistically significant (p > 0.05) and the best “control” model had a very low Nagelkerke R2 (0.098). These tests would seem to confirm the validity of our model, ruling out any potential effect of the control variables on the dependent variable.

5. Discussion and managerial implications

5.1 Theoretical contribution

Our findings suggest that many operational failures (glitches) in routine surgery are caused by sub-optimal coordination practices, supporting the idea that the occurrence of adverse events during surgery can be affected by team dynamics and other non-technical aspects as argued in previous studies (e.g. Healey et al., 2004; Yule et al., 2006; Siu et al., 2016; Gjeraa et al., 2017; Rubbio et al., 2019).

Specifically, Mirroring supports the theory that anticipatory behaviors of team members might improve the surgical performance by decreasing the likelihood of glitches. Such implicit coordination mechanisms permit practitioners to anticipate actions/needs of their colleagues and dynamically adjust their own behavior accordingly (Rico et al., 2008; Butchibabu et al., 2016). In so doing, they contribute to increasing the ability of the surgical team to manage the most critical surgical phases and to deal quickly with any issues that arise.

In addition, we found that a non-parsimonious use of explicit coordination, such as excessive verbal interactions (as shown by Silence), during surgery lead to a rise in surgical glitches. Redundant or unnecessary explicit coordination increases the noise and might cause misunderstandings in the operating room leading to distractions and thus less focus on the specific tasks (Wheelock et al., 2015; Singer et al., 2016; Keller et al., 2016). This may also have a negative impact on coordination during the most crucial moments of the surgical procedure or when the team has to make important decisions.

Our research also contributes to the literature investigating the role of team cohesion and research suggesting that “soft skills,” reflected in factors such as emotional intelligence, team personality, and shared mental models, can affect team performance (e.g. Lvina et al., 2018). From this perspective, our results confirm that team cohesion (as shown by variable Team Cohesion) lowers the probability that a team will experience a glitch during routine surgery. Cohesion among the team members creates a positive and collaborative working environment so that practitioners can act under a lower level of stress and cooperate more effectively (Arora et al., 2010). Consequently, team members tend to harmonize their behaviors during surgical operations and communicate more effectively (Burtscher and Manser, 2012; Sacks et al., 2015; Gjeraa et al., 2017). Such a favorable working environment enables practitioners to exploit their professional skills and react more promptly to problems, thus reducing the chances of glitches and obtaining a better care result.

Looking at the wider literature (Marks et al., 2001; Ilgen et al., 2005; Mathieu et al., 2008) and the factors that influence a team's operational performance, our empirical findings attribute a key role to team coordination dynamics in terms of “team process,” and team cohesion in terms of “team input” (Ilgen et al., 2005). In particular, the positive influence of implicit coordination and the negative impact of excessive explicit coordination on team performance would seem to be theoretically supported by the literature (e.g. Rico et al., 2008; Gorman, 2014). These results highlight the importance for managers and researchers to investigate the role of the coordination dynamics emerging during teamwork.

In line with previous work regarding team cohesion (e.g. Burtscher and Manser, 2012; Kovács et al., 2012; Gorman, 2014), our findings confirm the importance of soft skills, such as “team input,” for team outcomes. Although already examined in the literature, the importance of soft skills is still largely neglected during the composition of teams and job assignments in real business contexts (Mura et al., 2016; Donohue et al., 2018).

From a methodological perspective, this research also represents an early attempt to investigate coordination behaviors with sensor-based measurements of human behaviors. The paper thus provides a first empirical contribution to the call for investigations on the implications of Big Data and innovative data science methods for management research (George et al., 2014, 2016; Dubey et al., 2019). Indeed, novel approaches in organizational studies, based on new tools (e.g. wearable sensors, smartphones, social network platforms) and methods of analysis (e.g. machine learning, data mining), provide an opportunity to better answer past research questions and, most interestingly, to respond to new ones (George et al., 2016; Kache and Seuring, 2017).

However, exploiting such innovative approaches in real business settings is challenging and still quite limited (George et al., 2016; Chaffin et al., 2017). From this perspective, this research provides a first evidence on the use of wearable sensors for investigating issues in operations management. Specifically, our findings appear to endorse the usefulness of sociometric sensors for studying coordination behaviors and their relationship with operational performance in healthcare.

Our novel approach, which could also be combined and complemented with self-report and observational approaches, enables team dynamics to be detected and examined in more depth and could mitigate the limitations of previous approaches such as memory effect, subjectivity, and observers' influence. Therefore, through the use of Sociometric Badges, this work provides a first answer to the question of “how to” quantitatively investigate behavioral aspects in dynamic and complex operating environments (Croson et al., 2013; Brocklesby, 2016).

5.2 Managerial contribution

This study also provides hospital organizations with managerial insights for improving surgical team coordination. Specifically, as shown by our findings, practitioners can benefit from using implicit coordination behaviors during surgery, for example fostering deliberative and concise communication rather than reactive communication, and also fostering anticipatory behaviors (e.g. Butchibabu et al., 2016). Such behaviors could be enhanced through appropriate training and encouraged by the professional communities and top management commitment (Bendoly et al., 2015).

In addition, healthcare managers and practitioners should encourage a well-defined planning and distribution of surgical tasks, thus, reducing unnecessary verbal interactions during the surgical procedure. Similarly, concise communication by the surgical team leader can be useful to update the other members regarding the ongoing tasks and to focus on the surgical procedure. Periodic ward meetings could also be promoted allowing providers to discuss the issues affecting the operating teams during the surgical procedures. This would help limit unnecessary speaking or discussions during the operations both in terms of number and intensity, by solving the open issues deriving from different opinions among practitioners (Wheelock et al., 2015).

Finally, our evidence suggests that when selecting team members, healthcare managers should pay attention to the soft skills that facilitate team cohesion, and not just purely medical skills (Sacks et al., 2015; Donohue et al., 2018). Specifically, it could be valuable to find ways to identify and assess practitioners with similar values and “compatible” working behaviors that might enhance the internal cohesion. In line with this perspective, healthcare managers could plan team-building sessions to improve cohesion among all the staff (Amos et al., 2005). They could also promote a collaborative mindset for practitioners within the surgery unit and discourage a “culture of blame” (Khatri et al., 2009).

Although it is challenging to control for all the determinants of team coordination, these simple recommendations may help health managers in planning surgical activities and in training surgical staff in leadership, coordination, and collaboration skills.

6. Conclusions

This paper has investigated the relationships between team coordination and the related performance in routine surgery, using an innovative approach based on wearable sensors. We applied direct and quantitative measures for evaluating coordination behaviors in order to obtain more exhaustive measurements of individual and group behaviors, and to find more in-depth linkages between team coordination mechanisms and process performance. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first time that such relationships have been empirically explored using automated tools.

Specifically, our findings support our two hypotheses highlighting that: (1) the likelihood of adverse events and errors during routine surgeries is lower if the members of surgical teams coordinate implicitly rather than through explicit coordination; (2) the likelihood of adverse events and errors during routine surgeries is lower if the surgical team displays a higher level of team cohesion.

This research is not exempt from limitations that point out directions for future research. Drawing on a single case study, our results might be affected by the specific application and cultural context. This is a common issue for many behavioral studies and clearly limits the results from being generalized (Tröster et al., 2014).

Moreover, even though the number of monitored variables and related indicators was high, the study is clearly not conclusive. Other significant metrics describing coordination dynamics of surgery teams, which were not caught by sociometric measures, might exist. In addition, the control variables considered might be not exhaustive, and thus it might be desirable to consider a larger number of control variables for future research. Finally, although we checked for a reverse causality bias, due to privacy issues, the sociometric data do not permit the topic of the conversation to be identified, and, thus do prevent the dynamics and the effects of the “content” of verbal behaviors from being analyzed.

As a future development, it might be interesting to repeat this study in different cultural contexts to confirm our findings and to strengthen the practical recommendations. Culture tends to strongly shape individuals' values, language, demeanor, cognitive schemas, and preferences for how best to organize the work (Earley and Gibson, 2002). Therefore, coordination behavior patterns during surgery and their relationships with surgical performances might be different according to the cultural contexts, particularly if very dissimilar cultural contexts are compared (e.g. Western culture vs Eastern culture).

In addition, since different surgery settings may require different coordination mechanisms and interaction patterns (e.g. Manser, 2009), we suggest extending the research by reapplying this methodology to non-routine surgeries to enable a comparison of results between different operating contexts. It would also be interesting to empirically extend the effect of implicit and explicit coordination mechanisms on performance to other business settings, such as consulting, product development, as well as different healthcare settings, to verify whether the relationships revealed in our study can be generalized to other areas.

Lastly, more teamwork research enabled by wearable sensors would help to confirm the effectiveness and suitability of our approach.

Data collection

DataData sourceRole in the data analysisDescription
Coordination Behavior dataSociometric BadgesIndependent variablesIndividual and group behaviors of team members were directly collected (in real-time) through Sociometric Badges sensors
Cohesion perceptionsTeam Cohesion SurveyIndependent variablesThe perceptions of team cohesion of each working team were assessed through a “Team Cohesion” survey filled in by each participant of the team
Surgery & Patient CharacteristicsSurgical Register & Hospital information systemsControl VariablesThe main characteristics of the patient and surgery event were retrieved from the Surgical Register and from the Hospital Information Systems. To be noted that this class also includes the characteristics of surgical team (e.g. Practitioners' experience)
Surgical PerformanceSurgical RegisterDependent VariableAll the data related to the surgical glitches occurring during the surgeries were gathered from Surgical Register

Groups of independent variables

Groups of independent variablesData sourceDescriptions
Movement variablesSociometric BadgesThese variables are based on the accelerometer data and try to measure the movements of team members during the surgery from various viewpoints (e.g. level of physical activities, movement consistency, walking activity)
Speech variablesSociometric BadgesThese variables are based on the Front and Back microphone audio data and assess the speaking behaviors demonstrated by team members during the surgery
Interaction variablesSociometric BadgesThese variables are Social Network Analysis measurements (e.g. betweenness centrality, closeness, etc. – See Wasserman and Faust, 1994) based on the proximity of team members. The team member proximity is obtained through the proximity-Bluetooth data series
Group Flow variablesSociometric BadgesThese variables are built to understand the similarity in the behavior of the team members during the surgery and are theoretically founded on the mirroring (“mirror neurons”) theory (e.g. Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Kohler et al., 2002; Chartrand and Bargh, 1999). Indeed, these mirroring variables try to grasp the subconscious replication of nonverbal and/or paralanguage (voice) signals between team members (Iacoboni, 2009; Hall et al., 2019). Thus, the group flow variables are created for assessing the implicit coordination of teams. These variables are based on accelerometer, microphone audio, and proximity data
PerceptionSurveyThese variables evaluate the opinions of team members about the “team cohesion” through a survey, trying to assess how much team members share the same values/ideas and how much members feel to be involved in the specific surgery team

Independent variables

Variable groupVariableDescription
MovementBody MovementThe energy magnitude over the 3 movement axes measured by the accelerometer during the surgery. The team value was estimated as the average of the team members' value
Activity-WalkingIt measured the percentage of time that the badge wearer was moving/walking (based on the accelerometer's energy magnitude over the 3 axes of measurement). The team value was estimated as the average of the team members' value
Body Movement ConsistencyThe consistency of each badge's Body Movement (BM). Consistency ranges from 0 to 1, where 1 indicates no changes in activity levels, and 0 indicates the maximum amount of variation in activity levels. The team value was estimated as the average of the team members' value
Posture ActivityThe absolute angular velocity measured by the accelerometer during the surgery. The team value was estimated as the average of the team members' value
Posture ConsistencyThe consistency of each badge's Posture. Consistency ranges from 0 to 1, where 1 indicates no changes in posture activity levels, and 0 indicates the maximum amount of variation in posture activity levels. The team value was estimated as the average of the team members' value
SpeechSpeakingThe percentage of time in which the member was speaking. The independent variable, for each surgery, was determine as the average of the team members' values
Speaking overlapThe percentage of time in which the member was speaking while somebody else is speaking. The independent variable, for each surgery, was determine as the average of the team members' values
SilenceThe percentage of time in which the member and all others in his/her proximity were silent. This shows the proportion of time wherein a member did not participate to the conversation at all. The independent variable, for each surgery, was determined as the average of the team members' values
Turn-TakingThe number of changes of the members that are speaking during the surgery, normalized by time and by number of team members participating in the surgery. More “turns” in the conversation mean more involvement in the discussion by members. Hence, this index shows how much members are involved in the conversation
Audio ConsistencyThe consistency of each badge's speech amplitude. Consistency ranges from 0 to 1, where 1 indicates no changes in speech amplitude, and 0 indicates the maximum amount of variation in speech amplitude. The team value was estimated as the average of the team members' value
InteractionProximityBluetooth detections to determine the number of minutes that each badge was within strict interaction-distance of every other badge. The team value was estimated as the average of the team members' value
Betweenness CentralityBetweenness centrality (Wasserman and Faust, 1994) measured on the proximity network of team members. The team value was estimated as the average of the team members' value
Closeness CentralityCloseness centrality (Wasserman and Faust, 1994) measured on the proximity network of team members. The team value was estimated as the average of the team members' value
Degree CentralityDegree centrality (Wasserman and Faust, 1994) measured on the proximity network of team members. The team value was estimated as the average of the team members' value
CohesionCohesion (Wasserman and Faust, 1994) measured on the proximity network of team members
Group FlowMirroringIt assesses the similarity in the behaviors of team members during the surgery. It is estimated analyzing the speaking (recorded by the microphone), the body movement (recorded by the accelerometer) and the proximity (through the Bluetooth) data series. For every pair of team members it is possible to evaluate how similar their speaking data series and body movement data series are over time when they are within interaction-distance. These values range from 0 to 1, where 1 indicates that the data series of speaking and body movement follow identical patterns and 0 indicates no similarity. For every surgery, Mirroring was calculated as the average of the values for every pair of team members. This measure evaluates how similar the members' behavior in term of speech and movement patterns are, i.e. how much subconscious replication of non-verbal and paralanguage (voice) signals is happening. Therefore, this index can be used for estimating how much team members were anticipating actions/needs of their colleagues and dynamically adjusted their own behavior accordingly. Consequently, given the definition of implicit coordination, we considered this measurement as a proxy for assessing the level of implicit coordination of the surgery (see Table 1 and the related references for further details on the theory behind)
Group Flow - SpeechThe similarity in the speaking behaviors of team members during the surgery. It is estimated analyzing the speaking (recorded by the microphone), and the proximity data (through the Bluetooth) through the Sociometric Data Lab Software. For every pair of team members it is possible to evaluate how similar their speaking data series patterns are over time when they are within interaction-distance. These values range from 0 to 1, where 1 indicates that the data series of speaking follow identical patterns and 0 indicates no similarity. For every surgery, Group Flow-Speech was calculated as the average of the values for every pair of team members. This measure evaluates how similar the members' behavior in term of speech patterns are (see Table 1 for further details)
Group Flow - Body MovementThe similarity in the body behaviors of team members during the surgery. It is estimated analyzing the body movement (recorded by the accelerometer) and the proximity data (through the Bluetooth) through the Sociometric Data Lab Software. For every pair of team members it is possible to evaluate how similar their body movement data series are over time when they are within interaction-distance. These values range from 0 to 1, where 1 indicates that the data series of body movement follow identical patterns and 0 indicates no similarity. For every surgery, Group Flow-Speech was calculated as the average of the values for every pair of team members. This measure evaluates how similar the members' behavior in term of movement patterns are (see Table 1 for further details)
PerceptionTeam CohesionHow much the team members felt to be part of a cohesive team was assessed on a Likert scale from 1 to 5 using a specific six-item survey (Salas et al., 2015; Mathieu et al., 2015). It assesses both prospective social/interpersonal- and task-oriented aspects of the team as expected by team members before the beginning of the surgery. The six aspects evaluated are: feeling of belongingness among team members; feeling of unity and cohesion of team members; shared expectations about the task; shared views of the way of working; shared ideas about correct patterns of communication; feeling of positive “trusting climate”. The survey was filled in by practitioners for each different team they took part in. This independent variable, for every surgery, was determined as the average of the single team members' evaluations. This measure tries to assess how much team members share the same values/ideas and how much members feel to be involved in the specific surgery team
Team Cohesion varianceIt is based on the results obtained in the survey described for Team Cohesion. This independent variable, for every surgery, was determined as the variance of the team members' evaluations about team cohesion (rather than the average). This measure tries to assess the presence of strong differences between team members in the perception of team cohesion

Logistic Regression Model with Glitches ON/OFF as dependent variable

VariablesBS.E.SigExp(B)VIF
Logistic regression model
Silence−2.210.700.0090.111.19
Team Cohesion−1.710.660.0010.181.03
Mirroring−1.590.750.0340.211.17
Constant−2.760.730.0000.06
Model Summary
−2 Log likelihoodCox & Snell R2Nagelkerke R2
38.320.310.50

Correlations between dependent variable (Glitches ON/OFF), independent variables, and control variables

Note(s): **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). *. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)

Logistic Regression Model for Glitches ON/OFF with the control variables

Logistic regression model (with control variables)
VariablesBS.E.SigExp(B)VIF
Silence−2.610.920.0050.071.61
Team Cohesion−2.110.850.0070.121.07
Mirroring−1.860.940.0390.161.28
Surgery duration1.591.060.1344.921.20
People inside OR−0.580.660.3800.561.43
Operating Team0.040.970.9651.041.64
Type of breast surgery−0.870.810.2830.421.87
Patient age0.080.590.8871.091.16
Practitioners' experience−0.650.650.3160.521.39
Practitioners' experience variance1.260.810.1183.531.15
Team familiarity−0.670.700.3400.511.33
Constant0.664.690.8881.94
Appendix

Table A1

References

Abbott, T.E.F., Fowler, A.J., Dobbs, T.D., Harrison, E.M., Gillies, M.A. and Pearse, R.M. (2017), “Frequency of surgical treatment and related hospital procedures in the UK: a national ecological study using hospital episode statistics”, BJA: British Journal of Anaesthesia, Vol. 119, pp. 249-257.

Amos, M.A., Hu, J. and Herrick, C.A. (2005), “The impact of team building on communication and job satisfaction of nursing staff”, Journal for Nurses in Professional Development, Vol. 21 No. 1, pp. 10-16.

Ancarani, A., Di Mauro, C. and Giammanco, M. (2011), “Patient satisfaction, managers' climate orientation and organizational climate”, International Journal of Operations and Production Management, Vol. 31 No. 3, pp. 224-250.

Arora, S., Sevdalis, N., Nestel, D., Woloshynowych, M., Darzi, A. and Kneebone, R. (2010), “The impact of stress on surgical performance: a systematic review of the literature”, Surgery, Vol. 147, pp. 318-330.

Bamford, D. and Griffin, M. (2008), “A case study into operational team-working within a UK hospital”, International Journal of Operations & Production Management, Vol. 28 No. 3, pp. 215-237.

Barley, S.R. (1990), “Images of imaging: notes on doing longitudinal field work”, Organization Science, Vol. 1, pp. 220-247.

Ben-Menahem, S.M., Von Krogh, G., Erden, Z. and Schneider, A. (2016), “Coordinating knowledge creation in multidisciplinary teams: evidence from early-stage drug discovery”, Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 59, pp. 1308-1338.

Bendoly, E., van Wezel, W. and Bachrach, D.G. (2015), The Handbook of Behavioral Operations Management: Social and Psychological Dynamics in Production and Service Settings, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA.

Berry, J.C., Davis, J.T., Bartman, T., Hafer, C.C., Lieb, L.M., Khan, N. and Brilli, R.J. (2016), “Improved safety culture and teamwork climate are associated with decreases in patient harm and hospital mortality across a hospital system”, Journal of Patient Safety, pp. 1-7, doi: 10.1097/PTS.0000000000000251.

Bogdanovic, J., Perry, J., Guggenheim, M. and Manser, T. (2015), “Adaptive coordination in surgical teams: an interview study”, BMC Health Services Research, Vol. 15, p. 128.

Boyer, K.K. and Pronovost, P. (2010), “What medicine can teach operations: what operations can teach medicine”, Journal of Operations Management, Vol. 28 No. 5, pp. 367-371.

Brannick, M.T. and Prince, C. (1997), “An overview of team performance measurement”, Team Performance Assessment and Measurement, Psychology Press, East Sussex, UK, pp. 15-28.

Brocklesby, J. (2016), “The what, the why and the how of behavioural operational research—an invitation to potential sceptics”, European Journal of Operational Research, Vol. 249 No. 3, pp. 796-805.

Broekhuis, M. and Pieter van Donk, D. (2011), “Coordination of physicians' operational activities: a contingency perspective”, International Journal of Operations and Production Management, Vol. 31 No. 3, pp. 251-273.

Bucuvalas, J., Fronzetti Colladon, A., Gloor, P.A., Grippa, F., Horton, J. and Timme, E. (2014), “Increasing interactions in healthcare teams through architectural interventions and interpersonal communication analysis”, Journal of Healthcare Information Management: JHIM, Vol. 28 No. 4, pp. 58-65.

Burtscher, M.J. and Manser, T. (2012), “Team mental models and their potential to improve teamwork and safety: a review and implications for future research in healthcare”, Safety Science, Vol. 50, pp. 1344-1354.

Butchibabu, A., Sparano-Huiban, C., Sonenberg, L. and Shah, J. (2016), “Implicit coordination strategies for effective team communication”, Human factors, Vol. 58, pp. 595-610.

Cannon-Bowers, J.A., Salas, E. and Converse, S.A. (1993), “Shared mental models in expert team decision making”, Individual and Group Decision Making: Current Issues, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, USA, pp. 221-246.

Carayon, P., Kianfar, S., Li, Y., Xie, A., Alyousef, B. and Wooldridge, A. (2015), “A systematic review of mixed methods research on human factors and ergonomics in health care”, Applied Ergonomics, Vol. 51, pp. 291-321.

Catchpole, K., Mishra, A., Handa, A. and McCulloch, P. (2008), “Teamwork and error in the operating room: analysis of skills and roles”, Annals of surgery, Vol. 247, pp. 699-706.

Chaffin, D., Heidl, R., Hollenbeck, J.R., Howe, M., Yu, A., Voorhees, C. and Calantone, R. (2017), “The promise and perils of wearable sensors in organizational research”, Organizational Research Methods, Vol. 20, pp. 3-31.

Chang, H.T., Lin, C.C., Chen, C.H. and Ho, Y.H. (2017), “Explicit and implicit team coordination: development of a multidimensional scale”, Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, Vol. 45, pp. 915-929.

Chartrand, T.L. and Bargh, J.A. (1999), “The chameleon effect: the perception–behavior link and social interaction”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 76, p. 893.

Crawford, E.R. and LePine, J.A. (2013), “A configural theory of team processes: accounting for the structure of taskwork and teamwork”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 38, pp. 32-48.

Croson, R., Schultz, K., Siemsen, E. and Yeo, M.L. (2013), “Behavioral operations: the state of the field”, Journal of Operations Management, Vol. 31 Nos 1/2, pp. 1-5.

Deacon, M. and Cleary, M. (2013), “The reality of teamwork in an acute mental health ward”, Perspectives in Psychiatric Care, Vol. 49, pp. 50-57.

DeChurch, L.A. and Mesmer-Magnus, J.R. (2010), “Measuring shared team mental models: a meta-analysis”, Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, Vol. 14, p. 1.

Denton, B.T. (2013), Handbook of Healthcare Operations Management, Springer, New York, NY, Vol. 10, p. 978.

Dietz, A.S., Pronovost, P.J., Mendez-Tellez, P.A., Wyskiel, R., Marsteller, J.A., Thompson, D.A. and Rosen, M.A. (2014), “A systematic review of teamwork in the intensive care unit: what do we know about teamwork, team tasks, and improvement strategies?”, Journal of Critical Care, Vol. 29, pp. 908-914.

Donaldson, L. (2001), The Contingency Theory of Organizations, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.

Donohue, K., Katok, E. and Leider, S. (2018), The Handbook of Behavioral Operations, John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ, USA.

Dubey, R., Gunasekaran, A., Childe, S.J., Blome, C. and Papadopoulos, T. (2019), “Big data and predictive analytics and manufacturing performance: integrating institutional theory, resource based view and Big data culture”, British Journal of Management, Vol. 30 No. 2, pp. 341-361.

Earley, P.C. and Gibson, C.B. (2002), Multinational Work Teams: A New Perspective, Routledge, Mahwah, NJ, USA.

Easton, G.S. and Rosenzweig, E.D. (2015), “Team leader experience in improvement teams: a social networks perspective”, Journal of Operations Management, Vol. 37, pp. 13-30.

Ellway, B. (2016), “Design vs practice: how problematic call routing and rerouting restructures the call centre service system”, International Journal of Operations and Production Management, Vol. 36 No. 4, pp. 408-428.

Entin, E.E. and Serfaty, D. (1999), “Adaptive team coordination”, Human factors, Vol. 41 No. 2, pp. 312-325.

Espinosa, A., Lerch, F.J., Kraut, R.E., Salas, E. and Fiore, S.M. (2004), “Explicit vs. implicit coordination mechanisms and task dependencies: one size does not fit all”, Team Cognition: Understanding the Factors that Drive Process and Performance, American Psychological Association, Carnegie Mellon University, Washington, DC, USA, pp. 107-129.

Faraj, S. and Sproull, L. (2000), “Coordinating expertise in software development teams”, Management Science, Vol. 46, pp. 1554-1568.

Fernandes, A., Spring, M. and Tarafdar, M. (2018), “Coordination in temporary organizations: formal and informal mechanisms at the 2016 Olympics”, International Journal of Operations and Production Management, Vol. 38 No. 6, pp. 1340-1367.

Fisher, D.M., Bell, S.T., Dierdorff, E.C. and Belohlav, J.A. (2012), “Facet personality and surface-level diversity as team mental model antecedents: implications for implicit coordination”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 97, p. 825.

Forsyth, D.R. (2018), Group dynamics, Cengage Learning, Boston, MA, USA, ISBN 1337408859.

George, G., Haas, M.R. and Pentland, A. (2014), “Big data and management”, Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 57 No. 2, pp. 321-326.

George, G., Osinga, E.C., Lavie, D. and Scott, B.A. (2016), “Big data and data science methods for management research”, Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 59 No. 5, pp. 1493-1507.

Gjeraa, K., Mundt, A.S., Spanager, L., Hansen, H.J., Konge, L., Petersen, R.H. and Østergaard, D. (2017), “Important non-technical skills in video-assisted thoracoscopic surgery lobectomy: team perspectives”, The Annals of Thoracic Surgery, Vol. 104, pp. 329-335.

Gorman, J.C. (2014), “Team coordination and dynamics: two central issues”, Current Directions in Psychological Science, Vol. 23, pp. 355-360.

Gupta, D. (2007), “Surgical suites' operations management”, Production and Operations Management, Vol. 16, pp. 689-700.

Hall, J.A., Horgan, T.G. and Murphy, N.A. (2019), “Nonverbal communication”, Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 70, pp. 271-294.

Healey, A.N., Undre, S. and Vincent, C.A. (2004), “Developing observational measures of performance in surgical teams”, Quality and Safety in Health Care, Vol. 13, pp. i33-i40.

Healey, M.P., Vuori, T. and Hodgkinson, G.P. (2015), “When teams agree while disagreeing: reflexion and reflection in shared cognition”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 40, pp. 399-422.

Hosmer, D.W. Jr and Lemeshow, S. (2004), Applied Logistic Regression, John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, New Jersey, NJ.

Iacoboni, M. (2009), Mirroring People: The New Science of How We Connect with Others, Farrar, Straus and Giroux, New York, NY.

Ilgen, D.R., Hollenbeck, J.R., Johnson, M. and Jundt, D. (2005), “Teams in organizations: from input-process-output models to IMOI models”, Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 56, pp. 517-543.

Jebali, A., Alouane, A.B.H. and Ladet, P. (2006), “Operating rooms scheduling”, International Journal of Production Economics, Vol. 99, pp. 52-62.

Johnston, D., Diamant, A. and Quereshy, F. (2019), “Why do surgeons schedule their own surgeries?”, Journal of Operations Management, Vol. 65 No. 3, pp. 262-281.

Kache, F. and Seuring, S. (2017), “Challenges and opportunities of digital information at the intersection of big data analytics and supply chain management”, International Journal of Operations & Production Management, Vol. 37 No. 1, pp. 10-36.

Kayhan, V.O., Chen, Z.C., French, K.A., Allen, T.D., Salomon, K. and Watkins, A. (2018), “How honest are the signals? A protocol for validating wearable sensors”, Behavior Research Methods, Vol. 50 No. 1, pp. 57-83.

Keller, S., Tschan, F., Beldi, G., Kurmann, A., Candinas, D. and Semmer, N.K. (2016), “Noise peaks influence communication in the operating room. An observational study”, Ergonomics, Vol. 59, pp. 541-1552.

Khatri, N., Brown, G.D. and Hicks, L.L. (2009), “From a blame culture to a just culture in health care”, Health Care Management Review, Vol. 34 No. 4, pp. 312-322.

Kim, T., McFee, E., Olguin, D.O., Waber, B. and Pentland, A. (2012), “Sociometric badges: using sensor technology to capture new forms of collaboration”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 33, pp. 412-427.

Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M.A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V. and Rizzolatti, G. (2002), “Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons”, Science, Vol. 297, pp. 846-848.

Kovács, G., Tatham, P. and Larson, P.D. (2012), “What skills are needed to be a humanitarian logistician?”, Journal of Business Logistics, Vol. 33 No. 3, pp. 245-258.

Kozlowski, S.W. and Chao, G.T. (2012), “The dynamics of emergence: cognition and cohesion in work teams”, Managerial and Decision Economics, Vol. 33, pp. 335-354.

Kurmann, A., Keller, S., Tschan-Semmer, F., Seelandt, J., Semmer, N.K., Candinas, D. and Beldi, G. (2014), “Impact of team familiarity in the operating room on surgical complications”, World Journal of Surgery, Vol. 38, pp. 3047-3052.

Kuula, M. and Putkiranta, A. (2012), “Longitudinal benchmarking studies in operations management: lessons learned”, Benchmarking: An International Journal, Vol. 19 No. 3, pp. 358-373.

Leonard-Barton, D. (1990), “A dual methodology for case studies: synergistic use of a longitudinal single site with replicated multiple sites”, Organization Science, Vol. 1, pp. 248-266.

Levi, D. (2015), Group Dynamics for Teams, Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA, USA.

Lvina, E., Johns, G. and Vandenberghe, C. (2018), “Team political skill composition as a determinant of team cohesiveness and performance”, Journal of Management, Vol. 44 No. 3, pp. 1001-1028.

MacMillan, J., Entin, E.E. and Serfaty, D. (2004), Communication Overhead: The Hidden Cost of Team Cognition. Team Cognition: Process and Performance at the Inter and Intra-individual Level, American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, USA.

Malone, T.W. and Crowston, K. (1994), “The interdisciplinary study of coordination”, ACM Computing Surveys, Vol. 26, pp. 87-119.

Manser, T. (2009), “Teamwork and patient safety in dynamic domains of healthcare: a review of the literature”, Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica, Vol. 53, pp. 143-151.

Marks, M.A., Mathieu, J.E. and Zaccaro, S.J. (2001), “A temporally based framework and taxonomy of team processes”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 26, pp. 356-376.

Mathieu, J.E., Heffner, T.S., Goodwin, G.F., Salas, E. and Cannon-Bowers, J.A. (2000), “The influence of shared mental models on team process and performance”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 85, p. 273.

Mathieu, J., Maynard, M.T., Rapp, T. and Gilson, L. (2008), “Team effectiveness 1997-2007: a review of recent advancements and a glimpse into the future”, Journal of management, Vol. 34, pp. 410-476.

Mathieu, J.E., Kukenberger, M.R., D'innocenzo, L. and Reilly, G. (2015), “Modeling reciprocal team cohesion–performance relationships, as impacted by shared leadership and members' competence”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 100 No. 3, p. 713.

Mazzocco, K., Petitti, D.B., Fong, K.T., Bonacum, D., Brookey, J., Graham, S. and Thomas, E.J. (2009), “Surgical team behaviors and patient outcomes”, The American Journal of Surgery, Vol. 197, pp. 678-685.

McComb, S. and Simpson, V. (2014), “The concept of shared mental models in healthcare collaboration”, Journal of Advanced Nursing, Vol. 70, pp. 1479-1488.

McLaughlin, S.A. (2013), “Surgical management of the breast: breast conservation therapy and mastectomy”, Surgical Clinics, Vol. 93, pp. 411-428.

Miller, C.J., Kim, B., Silverman, A. and Bauer, M.S. (2018), “A systematic review of team-building interventions in non-acute healthcare settings”, BMC Health Services Research, Vol. 18, p. 146.

Mitchell, P. and Golden, R. (2012), Core Principles & Values of Effective Team-Based Health Care, National Academy of Sciences, Washington, DC, USA.

Morgan, S., Pullon, S. and McKinlay, E. (2015), “Observation of interprofessional collaborative practice in primary care teams: an integrative literature review”, International Journal of Nursing Studies, Vol. 52, pp. 1217-1230.

Mura, M., Lettieri, E., Radaelli, G. and Spiller, N. (2016), “Behavioural operations in healthcare: a knowledge sharing perspective”, International Journal of Operations & Production Management, Vol. 36 No. 10, pp. 1222-1246.

Nagelkerke, N.J.D. (1991), “A note on a general definition of the coefficient of determination”, Biometrika, Vol. 78, pp. 691-692.

Okhuysen, G.A. and Bechky, B.A. (2009), “Coordination in organizations: an integrative perspective”, The Academy of Management Annals, Vol. 3, pp. 463-502.

Olguín, D.O., Waber, B.N., Kim, T., Mohan, A., Ara, K. and Pentland, A. (2009a), “Sensible organizations: technology and methodology for automatically measuring organizational behavior”, Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, Part B: Cybernetics, IEEE Transactions on, Vol. 39, pp. 43-55.

Olguín, D.O., Gloor, P.A. and Pentland, A.S. (2009b), “Capturing individual and group behavior with wearable sensors”, Proceedings of the 2009 AAAI spring symposium on human behavior modeling, Vol. 9, pp. 67-74.

O'brien, R.M. (2007), “A caution regarding rules of thumb for variance inflation factors”, Quality and Quantity, Vol. 41, pp. 673-690.

Pentland, A.S. (2008), Honest Signals: How They Shape Our World, MIT press, Cambridge.

Rico, R., Sánchez-Manzanares, M., Gil, F. and Gibson, C. (2008), “Team implicit coordination processes: a team knowledge–based approach”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 33, pp. 163-184.

Riethmüller, M., Fernandez Castelao, E., Eberhardt, I., Timmermann, A. and Boos, M. (2012), “Adaptive coordination development in student anaesthesia teams: a longitudinal study”, Ergonomics, Vol. 55, pp. 55-68.

Rizzolatti, G. and Craighero, L. (2004), “The mirror-neuron system”, Annual Review of Neuroscience, Vol. 27, pp. 169-192.

Rosen, M.A., Dietz, A.S., Yang, T., Priebe, C.E. and Pronovost, P.J. (2014), “An integrative framework for sensor-based measurement of teamwork in healthcare”, Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association, Vol. 22 No. 1, pp. 11-18.

Rosen, M.A., DiazGranados, D., Dietz, A.S., Benishek, L.E., Thompson, D., Pronovost, P.J. and Weaver, S.J. (2018), “Teamwork in healthcare: key discoveries enabling safer, high-quality care”, American Psychologist, Vol. 73, p. 433.

Rubbio, I., Bruccoleri, M., Pietrosi, A. and Ragonese, B. (2019), “Digital health technology enhances resilient behaviour: evidence from the ward”, International Journal of Operations and Production Management, Vol. 39 No. 4, pp. 594-627.

Sacks, G.D., Shannon, E.M., Dawes, A.J., Rollo, J.C., Nguyen, D.K., Russell, M.M. and Maggard-Gibbons, M.A. (2015), “Teamwork, communication and safety climate: a systematic review of interventions to improve surgical culture”, BMJ Quality and Safety, Vol. 24 No. 7, pp. 458-467.

Salas, E., Grossman, R., Hughes, A.M. and Coultas, C.W. (2015), “Measuring team cohesion: observations from the science”, Human factors, Vol. 57 No. 3, pp. 365-374.

Schmutz, J. and Manser, T.D. (2013), “Do team processes really have an effect on clinical performance? A systematic literature review”, British Journal of Anaesthesia, Vol. 110, pp. 529-544.

Seelandt, J.C., Tschan, F., Keller, S., Beldi, G., Jenni, N., Kurmann, A. and Semmer, N.K. (2014), “Assessing distractors and teamwork during surgery: developing an event-based method for direct observation”, BMJ Quality and Safety, Vol. 23, pp. 918-929.

Senot, C., Chandrasekaran, A. and Ward, P.T. (2016), “Role of bottom up decision processes in improving the quality of health care delivery: a contingency perspective”, Production and Operations Management, Vol. 25 No. 3, pp. 458-476.

Shah, J. and Breazeal, C. (2010), “An empirical analysis of team coordination behaviors and action planning with application to human–robot teaming”, Human factors, Vol. 52, pp. 234-245.

Singer, S.J., Molina, G., Li, Z., Jiang, W., Nurudeen, S., Kite, J.G. and Berry, W.R. (2016), “Relationship between operating room teamwork, contextual factors, and safety checklist performance”, Journal of the American College of Surgeons, Vol. 223, pp. 568-580.

Siu, J., Maran, N. and Paterson-Brown, S. (2016), “Observation of behavioural markers of non-technical skills in the operating room and their relationship to intra-operative incidents”, The Surgeon, Vol. 14, pp. 119-128.

Smith-Jentsch, K.A., Kraiger, K., Cannon-Bowers, J.A. and Salas, E. (2009), “Do familiar teammates request and accept more backup? Transactive memory in air traffic control”, Human factors, Vol. 51, pp. 181-192.

Steiner, C.A., Karaca, Z., Moore, B.J., Imshaug, M.C. and Pickens, G. (2017), “Surgeries in hospital‐based ambulatory surgery and hospital inpatient settings, 2014”, Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. HCUP Statistical Brief, Vol. 223, pp. 1-18.

Stopczynski, A., Pietri, R., Pentland, A., Lazer, D. and Lehmann, S. (2014), Privacy in Sensor-Driven Human Data Collection: A Guide for Practitioners, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA.

Stout, R.J., Cannon-Bowers, J.A., Salas, E. and Milanovich, D.M. (1999), “Planning, shared mental models, and coordinated performance: an empirical link is established”, Human Factors, Vol. 41, pp. 61-71.

Sun, R., Marshall, D.C., Sykes, M.C., Maruthappu, M. and Shalhoub, J. (2018), “The impact of improving teamwork on patient outcomes in surgery: a systematic review”, International Journal of Surgery, Vol. 53, p. 171.

Thomas, C.L., Spitzmüller, C., Amspoker, A.B., Modi, V., Tran, T., Naik, A.D. and Hysong, S.J. (2018), “A systematic literature review of instruments to measure coordination”, Journal of Healthcare Management, Vol. 63, pp. e1-e18.

Tiferes, J., Bisantz, A.M. and Guru, K.A. (2015), “Team interaction during surgery: a systematic review of communication coding schemes”, Journal of Surgical Research, Vol. 195, pp. 422-432.

Tröster, C., Mehra, A. and van Knippenberg, D. (2014), “Structuring for team success: the interactive effects of network structure and cultural diversity on team potency and performance”, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Vol. 124, pp. 245-255.

Tschan, F., Seelandt, J.C., Keller, S., Semmer, N.K., Kurmann, A., Candinas, D. and Beldi, G. (2015), “Impact of case‐relevant and case‐irrelevant communication within the surgical team on surgical‐site infection”, British Journal of Surgery, Vol. 102, pp. 1718-1725.

Valentine, M.A., Nembhard, I.M. and Edmondson, A.C. (2015), “Measuring teamwork in health care settings: a review of survey instruments”, Medical care, Vol. 53, pp. e16-e30.

Vashdi, D.R., Bamberger, P.A. and Erez, M. (2013), “Can surgical teams ever learn? The role of coordination, complexity, and transitivity in action team learning”, Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 56, pp. 945-971.

Venkataraman, S., Fredendall, L.D., Taaffe, K.M., Huynh, N. and Ritchie, G. (2018), “An empirical examination of surgeon experience, surgeon rating, and costs in perioperative services”, Journal of Operations Management, Vol. 61 No. 1, pp. 68-81.

Wasserman, S. and Faust, K. (1994), Social Network Analysis: Methods and Applications, Cambridge University Press, New York, NY.

Wheelock, A., Suliman, A., Wharton, R., Babu, E.D., Hull, L., Vincent, C. and Arora, S. (2015), “The impact of operating room distractions on stress, workload, and teamwork”, Annals of surgery, Vol. 261, pp. 1079-1084.

Williams, A.L., Lasky, R.E., Dannemiller, J.L., Andrei, A.M. and Thomas, E.J. (2010), “Teamwork behaviours and errors during neonatal resuscitation”, Quality and Safety in Health Care, Vol. 19, pp. 60-64.

Yin, R.K. (2017), Case Study Research and Applications: Design and Methods, Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA, USA.

Yu, D., Blocker, R.C., Sir, M.Y., Hallbeck, M.S., Hellmich, T.R., Cohen, T. and Pasupathy, K.S. (2016), “Intelligent emergency department: validation of sociometers to study workload”, Journal of Medical Systems, Vol. 40 No. 3, p. 53.

Yule, S., Flin, R., Paterson-Brown, S. and Maran, N. (2006), “Non-technical skills for surgeons in the operating room: a review of the literature”, Surgery, Vol. 139, pp. 140-149.

Zaccaro, S.J., Rittman, A.L. and Marks, M.A. (2001), “Team leadership”, The Leadership Quarterly, Vol. 12 No. 4, pp. 451-483.

Further Reading

LePine, J.A., Piccolo, R.F., Jackson, C.L., Mathieu, J.E. and Saul, J.R. (2008), “A meta analysis of teamwork processes: tests of a multidimensional model and relationships with team effectiveness criteria”, Personnel Psychology, Vol. 61, pp. 273-307.

Corresponding author

Alessandro Stefanini can be contacted at: a.stefanini@ing.unipi.it

Related articles