Promising avenue or dead end street? A meta analytic review of the Forbes and Milliken model of board behaviour

Klaas Heemskerk (Amsterdam Institute for Social Science Research, Amsterdam, The Netherlands)

Corporate Governance

ISSN: 1472-0701

Article publication date: 3 June 2019

Issue publication date: 3 June 2019

1283

Abstract

Purpose

Over the past decades, growing interest in the behaviour of boards of directors has brought forth empirical studies on actual board behaviour. An important stream within this research followed the model proposed by Forbes and Milliken in 1999 in which the board processes, effort norms, cognitive conflict and the use of knowledge, are hypothesized to influence the performance of boards of directors. This paper aims to take stock of the results from this stream of research. The sometimes inconsistent results, and assumed methodological flaws of this research, leave open the question whether it makes sense to continue with this line of research.

Design/methodology/approach

Through a research synthesis of 17 primary studies on (parts of) the model proposed by Forbes and Milliken (1999), this question is addressed directly by clarifying what is known from the research done so far and by identifying possible distorting methodological moderators.

Findings

Strong empirical support is found for the effect of effort norms and the use of knowledge and skills on board task performance. The evidence for cognitive conflicts however was found to be inconclusive. Common method and respondent bias seem to be a lesser concern than often stated.

Research limitations/implications

Future studies should not only look closely at the construct validity of conflict, but should also have to account for the multidimensionality of conflicts and the interdependency and endogeneity in the relationship between behaviour and performance in boards.

Originality/value

This is the first paper that systematically integrates and reviews the empirical results of the research following the Forbes and Milliken model and sketches roads for future research on board behaviour.

Keywords

Citation

Heemskerk, K. (2019), "Promising avenue or dead end street? A meta analytic review of the Forbes and Milliken model of board behaviour", Corporate Governance, Vol. 19 No. 3, pp. 471-489. https://doi.org/10.1108/CG-01-2018-0017

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2019, Klaas Heemskerk.

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


1. Introduction

Over the past decade of corporate governance research, the insight that it is not so much the structural or formal characteristics that determine board performance, but rather social and behavioural aspects has been gaining ground (Ong and Wan, 2008; Sur, 2014). Several studies have been conducted, from different theoretical perspectives and with different methods, on the behavioural processes within boards of directors (Murphy and McIntyre, 2007; Neill and Dulewicz, 2010). Within this diverse stream of research, the concise and researchable model presented by Forbes and Milliken in 1999 has been the most promising avenue for the behavioural study of boards of directors. Huse considered it to be one of the major building blocks in understanding behavioural perspectives on boards, calling the article “a main point of reference for scholars wanting to explore behavioural dynamics in the boardroom” (Huse, 2009c, p. 64).

The findings in the research on board behaviour and board task performance using Forbes and Milliken, however, seem unclear and inconsistent. Although most researchers do conclude that the behavioural determinants are a better predictor of board performance compared to the formal and structural features, the effects of these behavioural determinants differ across studies (Huybrechts et al., 2016; Kumar and Zattoni, 2017). The effect of cognitive conflicts on board task performance for instance is sometimes not significant (Zona and Zattoni, 2007), negative (Minichilli et al., 2012) or positive (Bailey and Peck, 2011).

Despite the impact their widely cited article has had, these mixed results in subsequent empirical research and the methodological issues associated with survey research on organizational behaviour, left the promise of Forbes and Milliken (1999) to expand and refine our understanding of what makes boards effective unfulfilled (Huse et al., 2011).

Does it then make sense to pursue this rather disappointing line of research further or should we abandon the Forbes and Milliken model altogether? Before we throw out the good with the bad, we have to examine what the research into the Forbes and Milliken model of board performance (FM-Model) has contributed up till now and asses how the methodological issues it entails are to be addressed in future research on board behaviour.

Hence, a meta-analysis is performed on the effects drawn from empirical studies on (parts of) the FM-Model in investigating the three board processes it suggests: effort norms, cognitive conflict and the use of knowledge and skills. Meta-analysis allows us to test the hypotheses within the FM-Model empirically with larger datasets than those reported in any single primary study. Furthermore, meta-analytic research can identify possible methodological moderators on the relationships found between board processes and board performance in the studies under consideration. Three methodological issues associated with the research into the FM-Model are addressed in this meta-analysis: common method variance, single respondent bias and validity of constructs.

The effects of the use of knowledge and skills and effort norms on board task performance are found to be significant across studies, while the effects of cognitive conflicts are more heterogeneous (Heemskerk et al., 2017). Contrary to what is often thought, common method and response bias seem to pose no serious threat to the outcomes in research on board behaviour as their moderating effects were nonsignificant. The construct validity of cognitive conflict was found to have a strong moderating effect. Operationalisations of cognitive conflict aimed at the frequency of conflicts finds a negligibly small effect (ρ = 0.01), while operationalisations of cognitive conflict aimed at cognitive differences among board members finds a much larger effect (ρ = 0.37). This further emphasizes the complexity of conflicts as behavioural processes and calls for recognition of the multidimensionality and interdependency of conflicts in boards.

Section 2 discusses the hypotheses set forth by Forbes and Milliken (1999) and will further extend the hypotheses from the FM-Model with methodological moderators to guide further research. In Section 3, the method of meta-analysis is presented and discussed. Section 4 presents the results of the meta-analyses. The findings and the directions for further research are discussed in Sections 5 and 6 of this paper.

2. Theory and hypotheses

2.1 The Forbes and Milliken model of board performance

At the close of the twentieth century, Forbes and Milliken (1999) published their influential article on boards of directors as decision-making groups. They sought to understand the processes and behaviours involved in effective board performance. To do so, they saw a need to go beyond the demography-outcome approach and to incorporate the study of process-variables in the study of boards of directors in order to disentangle the predictions offered by multiple theoretical perspectives with regard to board demography. The effectiveness of boards in their model has two components. Control and service task performance constitute board task performance. They further identify three board processes that will influence board task performance: effort norms, cognitive conflict and the use of knowledge and skills. Effort norms are useful for ensuring preparation, participation and analysis. Cognitive conflict contributes to the leveraging of differences of perspective. Use of knowledge and skills is the ability to make use of the resources within the board in performing its tasks. The following hypotheses summarize the propositions as set forth by Forbes and Milliken (1999, p. 497):

H1.

Effort norms will be positively associated with effective board task performance.

H2.

Cognitive conflicts will be positively associated with effective board task performance.

H3.

The use of knowledge and skills will be positively associated with effective board task performance.

2.2 Methodological issues of the Forbes and Milliken model

The mixed results of the FM-model are usually attributed to methodological issues within the self-report survey research of team processes (Huse, 2009b). The key methodological issues in research using the FM-Model are:

  • common method variance (CMV);

  • relying on single respondents (mostly CEOs); and

  • validity of the constructs.

Almost all studies on the FM -model address the issue of CMV ex post or ex ante and it is often considered as a limitation of the research performed (Bailey and Peck, 2011). Likewise, the reliance on the CEO as the only respondent is an issue that is discussed in the limitations section of most studies (Zona and Zattoni, 2007). Not all studies reflect extensively on the construct validity of their concepts, but in particular with regard to conflict, the low construct validity is seen as a limitation of the research (Van Ees et al., 2008).

CMV or bias is often considered a major threat to the reliability of the correlations in self-report survey research design (Podsakoff et al., 2003). This threat of CMV, however, seems to be somewhat exaggerated. Spector (2006) therefore even considers it an urban legend and Fuller et al. (2016) argue that survey researchers face an unnecessary “presumption of guilt” in relation to CMV. Researchers testing for CMV using Harman’s one factor test or principal component analyses seldom find distorting effects of CMV in real-life data and the level of CMV in simulation data seems not to represent a grave threat to the reliability of study outcomes (Fuller et al., 2016). Therefore, the effect of board behaviour in studies that conduct ex-post analyses of CMV are not expected to differ from studies that do not explicitly address the issue of CMV:

H4.

Effort norms, cognitive conflict and the use of knowledge and skills will be equally positively associated with effective board task performance when the issue of CMV is addressed through ex-post analyses.

Survey studies in corporate governance are often based on the CEO as a single respondent (Huse, 2009a; Minichilli et al., 2009). Although the potential threat to the reliability of the data gained through one respondent is recognized, the difficulties in gaining access to multiple board members force studies to limit themselves to one respondent. The CEO then seems the most obvious option for they are in a better position than other board members to report on the functioning of the board (Huse, 2009a; Minichilli et al., 2009). The value creating board surveys (Huse, 2009b; Sellevoll et al., 2007) were conducted among CEOs, board chairs and board members and thus allow for a comparison between different respondents. CEOs are found to be in general less positive on boards’ task performance than chairs and other board members (Sellevoll et al., 2007: Table LXXII and LXXIII). Chairs and other board members also reported higher levels of cognitive conflict and preparation and involvement (Sellevoll et al., 2007: Table LXIII and LXVIII). As CEOs are more critical of board processes and board performance, we might expect that the effect of board processes would be smaller when measured through the CEO as the single respondent as compared to board chairs and other board members:

H5.

Effort norms, cognitive conflict and the use of knowledge and skills will be less positively associated with effective board task performance when measured through the CEO as single respondent.

Most discussions on the validity of the survey constructs in the FM-model concentrate on the operationalization of cognitive conflicts. There are broadly two ways in which task conflict is operationalized in studies following the FM-model. Some focus on the frequency of task conflicts while others are more concerned with the underlying cognition differences between board members. Forbes and Milliken themselves make use of Jehn’s (1995) definition of cognitive conflicts and point to Jehn’s four-item scale for task conflict as a reliable operationalization of cognitive conflict: “Using this scale, researchers could ask respondents to gauge, using Likert-type items, the frequency of conflicts about ideas and the extent of differences of opinion on the board” (Forbes and Milliken, 1999, p. 199). However, the studies that build on the FM model differ greatly in their operationalization of cognitive conflicts. Studies sometimes use different descriptions for cognitive conflicts: intragroup conflict (Garnes and Mathisen, 2014), critical debate (Minichilli et al., 2009) or task conflict (Heemskerk et al., 2015). In addition, studies using similar descriptions differ in the actual Likert-scale items used. Some follow Forbes and Milliken’s suggestion and ask for the frequency of conflicts or disagreement on several issues while others ask for differences in the way board members reason and argue. This difference in focus most likely also has an impact on the effect of task conflict.

Bendersky et al. (2010) differentiate between two different aspects of task conflicts, either as divergent task conflicts (discussing additional options and alternatives) or as convergent task conflicts (disagreement on a joint decision or solution). The cognition-based operationalization of cognitive conflict – how board members think and reason – seems to be more aimed at the divergent aspects of task conflict, while the frequency-based operationalization seems more concerned with the convergent aspects of task conflict. The performance-enhancing effects of cognitive conflict are attributed especially to the divergence of views and arguments (Forbes and Milliken, 1999, p. 494). This divergence in opinions, solutions and arguments is more likely to be found in the cognition-based operationalization than in the one that focuses more on the frequency of conflicts. We thus might expect a stronger effect of cognitive conflict on board task performance in studies using a cognition-based operationalization compared to studies using a frequency-based operationalization of cognitive conflict:

H6.

Cognitive conflict will be more positively associated with effective board task performance when survey items focus on cognition rather than frequency of conflicts.

3. Meta-analytic methods

3.1 Inclusion criteria and effect size

To review the empirical evidence on the hypotheses within the FM-Model and to test for the additional hypotheses on methodological moderators, meta-analyses were conducted as a quantitative method for combining evidence from different studies (Schmidt and Hunter, 2014). First, all studies citing the 1999 Forbes and Milliken article were searched using Google Scholar (1,485 articles) and Web of Science (361 articles) up until August 16, 2016. Those articles that empirically tested the hypothesis of the FM-Model were selected by screening the abstracts of these articles. Inclusion criteria were primarily the explicit use of (part of) the FM-Model to quantitatively examine board effectiveness. Qualitative studies using the FM-Model, quantitative studies on board behaviour not using the FM-Model or quantitative studies using the FM-Model but not inquiring board effectiveness were not included. Three quantitative studies missing effect sizes on the FM-Model relations were not included (Mande et al., 2013; Marchewka, 2015; Zona, 2015). The final sample consisted of 17 primary studies (Appendix 1). All included studies were coded using a coding guide in line with the suggestions of Cooper (2009).

The Pearson correlation coefficient (r) for board processes and board task performance was used as effect size to integrate results from the included studies. The correlation coefficient is commonly used in meta-analyses for it is a scale-free measure of linear association. In total, 70 correlations between board processes and board outcomes were obtained. As previously elaborated, the various studies differ in the way they classify board tasks. Some treat board task performance as one monolithic dependent variable, while others use different twofold or threefold classification in their operationalization of board task performance. For the inclusion of the board task performance variables, the classification of board task performance as service and control in the FM-Model was followed and when possible a distinction was made between the control or monitoring board task performance and the service or advisory task of board directors. Additional board tasks outside the scope of the FM-Model, such as the performance of the resource-dependency role of the board (Wan and Ong, 2005) or the networking role of the board (Zona and Zattoni, 2007), were excluded from the analyses (Forbes and Milliken, 1999, pp. 501-502). The included independent and dependent variables and the effect sizes extracted are presented and accounted in Appendix 1.

3.2 Meta-analysis techniques

For the meta-analysis of the main effects, a random-effects model was used. Sampling variance was calculated using the formula in Hedges and Olkin (1985): Var(e) = (1 − ρ2)2/N − 1. As suggested by Schmidt and Hunter (2014) the population correlation (ρ) in the calculated sampling variance was estimated by the mean observed correlation rather than the individual study correlations; for the latter might lead to a negative bias (Aguinis, 2001). Study-level variance was estimated using Hunter and Schmidt’s (2014) estimator of heterogeneity variance (τ2 = max{0,(Q − k)/∑Wi}). Calculations were performed directly on the correlation coefficients (Schmidt and Hunter, 2014). No artefact corrections were performed since the study characteristics are treated as moderating effects in the analyses.

For the moderator analyses, a hierarchical two-stage approach was followed. First, a mixed effects model, using Hunter and Schmidt’s (2014) estimator of heterogeneity variance, was calculated to test the moderator hypotheses of the dummy-coded moderators. Second, a subgroup analysis of fixed effects was conducted, in which the calculating of the meta-analytic mean correlations and confidence intervals were adopted from Hedges and Olkin (1985). The mean correlation coefficient (r) was calculated using Fisher’s (inverse) transformation (z = ½Ln((r + 1)/(r − 1)) to correct for skewness in the effect size distribution. Mean ρ was estimated by weighting the effect sizes as z-scores by their sample size (mean z = ∑(n − 3)z/∑(n − 3)) and converted to mean rho (ρ) using Fisher’s transformation. The random and mixed effects models were calculated in R version 3.2.4 using the metafor package version 1.9-9 (R Core Team, 2016; Viechtbauer, 2010). Fixed effect subgroup calculations were done by hand using the suggestions by Hedges and Olkin (1985).

4. Results

Table I presents meta-analytic evidence that further bears on the broad relationships between board processes and board task performance. Analyses of the funnel plots for these effects, presented in Figure 1 suggest no distorting effect of publication bias (Egger et al., 1997). As most studies compare the effects of board processes in the FM-Model with the formal characteristics of boards (size, composition, CEO-duality, etc.), it is indeed not expected that non-significant findings will not be published.

Support was found for H1, which proposed a positive relationship between effort norms and board task performance; a positive relationship was found between effort norms and overall board task performance (ρ = 0.41, CI = 0.36 – 0.46), as well as between effort norms and both service and control task performance (ρ = 0.41, CI = 0.34 – 0.48 and ρ = 0.35, CI = 0.29 – 0.42, respectively). Effort norms thus seem relevant behavioural determinants of board task performance.

The meta-analysed effect of cognitive conflict on overall board task performance is rather small (ρ = 0.19, CI = 0.19 – 0.29). Given the value of Q relative to the number of studies included (I2 > 90 per cent[1]) indicating evidence of considerable heterogeneity, the effect is too small to firmly confirm H2, which predicted a positive association between cognitive conflicts and effective board task performance. The separate effects of cognitive conflict on service task performance (ρ = 0.14, CI = 0.05 – 0.24) and control task performance (ρ = 0.20, CI = 0.09 – 0.30) do not support the idea that cognitive conflicts have a different effect on specific board tasks. Support was found for H3, which proposed a positive association between the use of knowledge and skills and effective board task performance. This positive relationship was found between the use of knowledge and skills and overall board task performance (ρ = 0.39, CI = 0.31 – 0.48), as well as between the use of knowledge and skills and both board service task performance and board control task performance (ρ = 0.40, CI = 0.28 – 0.52 and ρ = 0.51, CI = 0.38 – 0.64, respectively).

4.1 Methodological moderating effects

The Q-values reported in Table I suggest the existence of moderating variables. To test H4, which proposed no moderating effect of ex post controlling for CMV-issues on the relation between board processes and board task performance, the included studies were divided into two categories: studies in which the possibility of CMV was assessed through Harman’s one factor test or other statistical post analyses (Podsakoff et al., 2003) and studies in which such analyses were not reported. The two studies with a multi-method research design were excluded from the analyses (McNulty et al., 2013; Ranasinghe et al. 2015). For all three board processes, the moderator model for ex-post CMV controlling was non-significant (Qm effort norms = 1,58; p > 0.05, Qm cognitive conflict = 0,35; p > 0.05 and Qm use of knowledge and skills = 0,78; p > 0.05). Results of the meta-analyses for both categories are summarized in Table III, which further support H4 as the confidence intervals for all three board processes overlap in both cases Table II.

To test for the moderating effect of relying on the CEO as single respondent versus the use of other respondents, the studies were grouped by those targeting the CEO and those who surveyed other or multiple respondents (board chairs, non-executive directors, board secretaries, etc.). Studies that combined data gathered from both CEOs and (supervisory) board chairs were thus classified as using other respondents (Van Ees et al., 2008).

The type of respondent as a moderator was found not significant for all three board processes (Qm effort norms = 0.07; p > 0.05, Qm cognitive conflict = 1.21; p > 0.05, Qm use of knowledge and skills = 0.00; p > 0.05). The results of the meta-analyses presented in Table IV further illustrate that, contrary to H5, there is no significant difference in effects of board processes in studies only targeting the CEO compared to studies using other or multiple respondents Table III.

H6 predicted that cognitive conflict is more positively associated with effective board task performance in studies that used cognition-based items rather than frequency-based items. To test for the moderating effect of operationalization of cognitive conflict, the studies were grouped by those primarily inquiring the frequency of conflicts on different subjects and those that asked about the differences in reasoning and arguing within the board. An overview of all survey items on cognitive conflict in the included studies can be found in Appendix 2. One study did not report survey items and was excluded from the analysis (Ranasinghe et al., 2015). The moderator model was indeed found significant (Qm = 27.49; p < 0.01) and the differences in meta-analysed effect size, as shown in Table IV, are indeed quite large (ρ = 0.01, CI = 0.03 − 0.05 for frequency-based studies ρ = 0.37, CI = 0.32 − 0.41 for cognition-based studies). While both sets of studies remain heterogenic, the level of heterogeneity is more equally divided and less excessively high (I2 < 90 per cent) (Table IV).

5. Discussion and conclusions

The purpose of this meta-analysis is twofold. First, the aim is to take stock of the research on the FM-Model and to estimate the effect of effort norms, cognitive conflicts and the use of knowledge and skills on board task performance. A second objective is to assess the moderating effect of the methodological issues within self-report survey research on the FM-Model.

Regarding the first objective, the meta-analyses performed demonstrate that there is enough evidence for the significant positive effect of both effort norms and the use of knowledge and skills on board task performance. The meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive conflict on board task performance, however, demonstrated considerable heterogeneity. This further underlines the severe differences in findings between studies and makes it hard to draw conclusions. Some empirical studies have found that the impact of cognitive conflict depends on the board task and have suggested that cognitive conflict has a more positive impact on the service task of a board than on its control task (Zattoni et al., 2015, Heemskerk et al. 2015). However, the similarities found in the meta-analysis for the effect on board control task performance, and board service task performance does not support this supposition.

The inconclusive findings on cognitive conflict reflect the ongoing debate in the team effectiveness literature on the possible positive effects of task conflicts. At the end of the previous century, Jehn (1995) found that task conflicts increased team performance and Amason (1996) showed that task conflict was positively linked to decision-quality in TMTs. However, this positive perception on task conflict began to tilt when De Dreu and Weingart (2003) detected no positive effect of task conflict on team performance in their meta-analysis. They only left room for positive effects of conflicts “under very specific circumstances” (De Dreu and Weingart, 2003, p. 748). Two recent meta-analyses on conflict and team performance provide a more complex and detailed understanding of these specific circumstances. First, the effect seems to be context dependent; task conflicts produce more positive outcomes for strategic and complex decision-making and more negative outcomes for routine tasks (O'Neill et al., 2013). Second, the effect seems to depend on the organizational level at which they occur; the higher up in the organizational hierarchy, the more positive the effects of task conflicts are (De Wit et al., 2012). This suggests that boards might benefit from task conflict, because they are typically involved in non-routine decision-making at the apex of organizational hierarchy (Heemskerk et al., 2017).

The heterogeneity in the effects of cognitive conflicts might also partly be due to a limitation of this meta-analysis. There is a certain variation in the operationalization of board effectiveness between the various included studies (financial risk taking, commitment, etc.) while the measurement of performance has turned out to be a moderator in meta-analyses of the effect of task conflict on team performance (De Wit et al., 2012).

In answer to why research on board processes is still limited in number, Huse states that: “Response rates tend to be very low, respondent and common method biases are likely to occur, […] and concept validity is often weak” (Huse, 2009b, p. 368). The second purpose of this meta-analysis is to assess the effect of these methodological issues: CMV, relying on a single respondent and validity of concepts.

Research on the FM-Model typically uses the same method to measure the dependent and independent variables. It is often believed that such research leads to inflated correlations through CMV or bias. The risk of CMV however seems grossly exaggerated. Studies that control for CMV almost never find a confounding effect of CMV and, in simulations, the effect is negligible (Fuller et al., 2016; Spector, 2006). The moderator analysis for ex post CMV controlling confirms this. We found no significant differences in effect between studies that do and studies that do not check for CMV afterwards. Moreover, in all the studies controlling for CMV, it had no distorting effect on the outcomes. CMV seems to be less of a concern in FM-Model research than often assumed.

Another regularly criticized methodological issue in the research on the FM-Model is the reliance on the CEO as a single respondent in some of the research (Huse, 2009a). The choice of the CEO as the single respondent is often motivated by the difficult accessibility of non-executive board members and justified by stating that the CEO is the only person that can properly assess both the ins and outs of the organization as well as the functioning of the board of directors (Minichilli et al., 2012; Heemskerk et al., 2015). Contrary to our hypotheses, the moderator model comparing studies that relied on the CEO as single respondent with studies that relied on other or multiple respondents was not significant. The number of respondents targeted within the board thus does not seem to influence the effect found for the board processes on board effectiveness. This makes the choice to question only the CEO more defensible, but it remains interesting to identify – not at the aggregate, but at the micro level – the differences in responses between CEOs and other board members.

Furthermore, it is interesting to see that there is a notable difference between the cognition-oriented and frequency-oriented operationalizations of cognitive conflict. This points to the need to come to a shared operationalization of conflicts in board process studies to avoid comparing apples and oranges. Upon closer inspection, operationalizations of conflict appear sloppy and simplistic in both approaches. Frequency-based items often too simply examine if there are frequent conflicts and disagreements among directors (Minichilli et al., 2009) leaving it unclear to what extent disagreement is cognitive or task-oriented. Cognition-based items differ along studies; in one study, for instance, one of the three items on conflict asked respondents’ opinion on the statement: “A good director doesn’t wait too long to resign” (Van Ees et al. 2008, p. 91). This is an item not used in other studies and it does not seem to adequately measure conflict. It is also troublesome to notice that exactly the same items are sometimes excluded, based on reliability analysis, in one study, yet not excluded in another. For instance, the item “The board is able to reach collectively shared decisions following a full and frank debate” was excluded based on a factor analysis in Bailey and Peck (2011, p. 14) and was not excluded in McNulty et al. (2013) based on a principal component analysis.

6. Directions for future research

Almost two decades since Forbes and Milliken, strong evidence has been accumulated for the positive effect of board processes such as effort norms and the use of knowledge and skills. Unfortunately the discussion above illustrate that we still lack understanding of what is arguably the most interesting and promising of all board processes: cognitive conflict. The actual methodological issues of the FM-model are far more fundamental than the overestimated threats of common method and respondent bias. Empirical research into board behaviour and performance could therefore greatly benefit from further research that changes course from research on conflicts within boards done so far in two important ways.

First, to clarify the effect of conflicts within boards, it is crucial for future research to recognize the multidimensionality of conflicts. The history of the conflict classification as sketched by Jehn (2014) shows a tendency toward a tripartite distinction between task conflicts, process conflicts and relationship conflicts. Task conflicts are focussed on the content of the task at hand, process conflicts are focussed on how tasks should be accomplished, and relationship conflicts are driven by interpersonal incompatibilities resulting in tension, animosity and annoyance among team members (Jehn, 1995). An important additional distinction typical for conflicts within boards that needs to be included in future research on the FM-Model is the difference between intragroup conflicts, debate and arguments among non-executive board members, and debate and arguments between non-executive board members and the CEO and other executive directors (Heemskerk et al., 2017). More attention to the multi-dimensionality of conflicts and more rigour in the operationalization of conflicts in future research will bring more insight into the workings of conflicts in boards.

Second, future research could further address the limitations through a longitudinal research design in which board’s behavioural processes and task performance are measured at multiple times. This longitudinal approach would enable us to clarify the interdependencies between different board processes and to unravel the endogeneity in the causal relations between processes and performance (Boyce et al., 2015; Byers et al. 2015). Although a longitudinal study is more resource intensive, difficult and risky, it is a necessary step to move beyond oversimplified research designs in board effectiveness studies (Van Ees et al., 2009). The FM-model is still a promising avenue for gaining insight in the inner working of boards, but to live up to its promise, we need to take the next step and improve the rigour and relevance of the behavioural study of boards.

Figures

Funnel plots of the main effects

Figure 1

Funnel plots of the main effects

Meta-analyses results

Relation k N Mean r Mean ρ SE ρ 95% confidence interval Q test of homogeneity
Overall board performance
Effort norms 16 3793 0.42 0.41 0.026 0.36; 0.46 54.37**
Cognitive conflict 16 3943 0.21 0.19 0.052 0.09; 0.29 177.18**
Use of knowledge and skills 12 2552 0.41 0.39 0.046 0.31; 0.48 85.15**
Service task performance
Effort norms 9 2705 0.43 0.41 0.036 0.34; 0.48 42.80**
Cognitive conflict 10 2990 0.16 0.14 0.048 0.05; 0.24 69.07**
Use of knowledge and skills 7 1906 0.41 0.40 0.061 0.28; 0.52 67.84**
Control task performance
Effort norms 9 2705 0.36 0.35 0.035 0.29; 0.42 36.44**
Cognitive conflict 10 2990 0.21 0.20 0.054 0.09; 0.30 92.21**
Use of knowledge and skills 7 1906 0.53 0.51 0.064 0.38; 0.64 98.24**
Notes:

k = number of effect sizes; N = total sample size; r = mean estimate of uncorrected correlations; rho = mean estimate of corrected population correlation; SE rho = estimated standard error of rho. Overall board task performance for studies reporting separate effects for service task performance and control task performance was calculated by computing their mean using Fisher’s (inverse) z-transformation.

*

p < 0.05;

**p < 0.01

Meta-analysis of the moderating effect of ex post CMV controlling on board task performance

Relation k N Mean r Mean ρ 95% confidence interval Q test
No ex post CMV testsa
Effort norms 7 1571 0.41 0.37 0.33; 0.41 29.59**
Cognitive conflict 7 1721 0.18 0.11 0.07; 0.16 101.06**
Use of knowledge and skills 3 675 0.51 0.49 0.40; 0.58 4.12
Ex post CMV testb
Effort norms 7 1897 0.47 0.44 0.40; 0.47 10.79
Cognitive conflict 7 1897 0.26 0.14 0.09; 0.18 74.55**
Use of knowledge and skills 7 1897 0.43 0.42 0.38; 0.45 38.74**
Notes:

k = number of effect sizes; N = total sample size; r = mean estimate of uncorrected correlations; rho = mean estimate of corrected population correlation.

*

p < 0.5;

**p < 0.1;

Meta-analysis of the moderating effect of respondents on board task performance

Relation k N Mean r Mean ρ 95% confidence interval Q test
CEO respondenta
Effort norms 7 2380 0.42 0.39 0.35; 0.42 29.16**
Cognitive conflict 7 2380 0.13 0.11 0.07; 0.15 34.37**
Use of knowledge and skills 5 1581 0.40 0.41 0.36; 0.45 37.84**
Other respondent(s)b
Effort norms 9 1413 0.42 0.42 0.38; 0.47 21.57**
Cognitive conflict 9 1563 0.27 0.17 0.12; 0.22 156.85**
Use of knowledge and skills 7 971 0.42 0.40 0.35; 0.45 45.83**
Notes:

k = number of effect sizes; N = total sample size; r = mean estimate of uncorrected correlations; rho = mean estimate of corrected population correlation.

*

p < 0.5;

**p < 0.1;

Meta-analysis of the moderating effect of the item types of cognitive conflict on board task performance

Relation k N Mean r Mean ρ 95% confidence interval Q test
Cognitive conflict
Frequency-based itemsa 8 2467 0.04 0.01 0.03; 0.05 42.82**
Cognition-based itemsb 7 1292 0.41 0.37 0.32; 0.41 35.09**
Notes:

k = number of effect sizes; N = total sample size; r = mean estimate of uncorrected correlations; rho = mean estimate of corrected population correlation.

*

p < 0.5;

**

p < 0.1;

Effect sizes of included studies

Author N Setting Process variables included Board performance variables included Effect sizes (r)
1. Wan and Ong (2005) 424 Listed firms in Singapore EN
CC
UKS
A. Service role
B. Monitoring role
EN × A: 0.44
EN × B: 0.57
CC × A: 0.38
CC × B: 0.47
UKS × A: 0.56
UKS × B: 0.58
2. Zona and Zattoni (2007) 301 Large manufacturing firms in Italy EN
CC
UKS
A. Service
B. Monitoring
EN × A: 0.50
EN × B: 0.33
CC × A: 0.00
CC × B: 0.00
UKS × A: 0.25
UKS × B: 0.39
3. van Ees et al. (2008) 136 Large firms in the Netherlands EN
CC
UKS
A. Strategy
B. Monitoring
EN × A: 0.19
EN × B: 0.38
CC × A: 0.02
CC × B: 0.45
UKS × A: 0.24
UKS × B: 0.57
4. Minichilli et al. (2009) 301 Large industrial firms in Italy EN (commitment)
CC (critical debate)
A. Service: Advisory task, networking task and strategic participation
B. Control: Behavioural control, output control and strategy control.
EN × A: 0.46a
EN × B: 0.41a
CC × A: 0.08a
CC × B: 0.01a
5. Gnan and Zattoni (2009) 498 Small firms in Norway EN (commitment)
CC
A. Advisory task
B. Control task
EN × A: 0.26b
EN × B: 0.19b
CC × A: 0.27b
CC × B: 0.30b
6.Scarborough et al. (2010) 135 Firms with a corporate secretary in the US EN Board activism EN: 0.56
7. Bailey and Peck (2011) 104 Publicly traded firms in the United Stated EN
CC
UKS
Board task performance EN: 0.53
CC: 0.69
UKS: 0.50
8. Namoga (2011) 86 Public and private enterprises on Fiji and Solomon Islands EN
CC
UKS
A. Service role performance
B. Monitoring and control role performance
EN × A: 0.62
EN × B: 0.17
CC × A: 0.44
CC × B: 0.29
UKS × A: 0.45
UKS × B: 0.73
9. Minichilli et al. (2012) 535 Listed and large firms in Norway and Italy EN
CC
UKS
A. Advisory task performance
B. Control task performance
EN × A: 0.40
EN × B: 0.36
CC × A: 0.01
CC × B: 0.01
UKS × A: 0.34
UKS × B: 0.26
10. Garnes and Mathisen (2014) 307 Collaborative tourist organisations in Norway EN
CC
Board commitment EN 0.44
CC–0.30
11. McNulty et al. (2013) 141 Large firms in the United Kingdom EN
CC
UKS
Financial risk-taking EN: 0.22c
CC: 0.20c
UKS: −0.02c
12. Msweli and Singh (2014) 289 State owned firms in South Africa EN
CC
UKS
Board decision quality EN: 0.45
CC: 0.54
UKS: 0.60
13. Zattoni et al. (2015) 488 SMEs in Norway EN
CC
UKS
A. Strategy task performance
B. Monitoring task performance
EN × A: 0.50
EN × B: 0.37
CC × A: 0.10
CC × B: 0.01
UKS × A: 0.65
UKS × B: 0.66
14. Heemskerk et al. (2015) 148 Educational organisations in the Netherlands EN
CC (task conflict)
UKS
A. Advisory task performance
B. Control task performance
EN × A: 0.39
EN × B: 0.39
CC × A: 0.33
CC × B: 0.32
UKS × A: 0.32
UKS × B: 0.41
15. Ranasinghe et al. (2015) 184 Listed firms in Australia EN
CC
UKS
Monitoring of financial reporting EN: 0.33
CC: 0.10
UKS: 0.30
16. Mostert et al. (2015) 285 Banks and insurance companies in the Netherlands CC A. Board advice provision
B. Board monitoring
CC × A: −0.09
CC × B: 0.21
17. Zona (2016) 109 Family firms in Italy EN
CC
UKS
Board performance EN: 0.61
CC: 0.21
UKS: 0.43
Notes:

EN = effort norms, CC = cognitive conflict and UKS = the use of knowledge and skills.

aBoth board roles were measured as three separate variables. Although one should in general not add up correlations, the similarity of the three board roles both in concept as in outcome makes it justifiable to compute a mean r using Fisher’s z-transformation.

bEffect sizes in this study were reported as βs within a multiple regression analysis. Estimates of effect sizes as r are calculated using the transformation suggested by Peterson and Brown (2005): r = 0.99β + 0.04λ + 0.2η with λ = 1 (for β > 0) and η = 1 (for the average intercorrelation of the predictor variable set is > 0.18).

cEffect sizes of the three (inverse) proxies of financial risk-taking during the credit crisis: ΔCash&Equivalents, ΔNetCash and ΔFinancialSlack were reported separately. Given the similarities, conceptual as well as numerical, a mean effect size for r was computed using Fisher’s z-transformation

Note

1.

I² describes the percentage of variation across studies due to heterogeneity rather than chance. It was calculated using I² = ((Q − df)/Q) * 100%.

Appendix 1

Table AI

Appendix 2. Overview of survey items on cognitive conflict

Figure A1

References

References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.

Aguinis, H. (2001), “Estimation of sampling variance of correlations in meta‐analysis”, Personnel Psychology, Vol. 54 No. 3, pp. 569-590.

Amason, A.C. (1996), “Distinguishing the effects of functional and dysfunctional conflict on strategic decision-making: resolving a paradox for top management teams”, Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 39 No. 1, pp. 123-148.

*Bailey, B.C. and Peck, S.I. (2011), “Board processes, climate and the impact on board task performance”, paper presented at the First International Conference on Engaged Management Scholarship, June 2-5, Cleveland, OH, available at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=1852596 (accessed 21 October 2014).

Bendersky, C., Behfar, K., Weingart, L.R., Todorova, G., Bear, J. and Jehn, K. (2010), “Revisiting the dimensions of intra-group conflict: theoretical and psychometric construct refinement”, paper presented at the IACM 23rd Annual Conference. Boston, MA, June 24-27, available at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=1611845 (accessed 2 December 2014).

Boyce, A.S., Nieminen, L.R.G., Gillespie, M.A., Ryan, A.M. and Denison, D.R. (2015), “Which comes first, organizational culture or performance? A longitudinal study of causal priority with automobile dealerships”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 36 No. 3, pp. 339-359.

Byers, T., Anagnostopoulos, C. and Brooke-Holmes, G. (2015), “Understanding control in nonprofit organisations: moving governance research forward?”, Corporate Governance: The International Journal of Business in Society, Vol. 15 No. 1, pp. 134 -145.

Cooper, H. (2009), Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis: A Step-by-step Approach, 4th ed., Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA.

De Dreu, C. and Weingart, L. (2003), “Task versus relationship conflict, team member satisfaction, and team effectiveness: a meta-analysis”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 88 No. 4, pp. 741-749.

De Wit, F., Greer, L. and Jehn, K. (2012), “The paradox of intragroup conflict: a meta-analysis”, The Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 97 No. 2, pp. 360-390.

Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M. and Minder, C. (1997), “Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test”, BMJ (Clinical Research ed.), Vol. 315 No. 7109, pp. 629-634.

Forbes, D.P. and Milliken, F.J. (1999), “Cognition and corporate governance: understanding boards of directors as strategic decision-making groups”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 24 No. 3, pp. 489-505.

Fuller, C.M., Simmering, M.J., Atinc, G., Atinc, Y. and Babin, B.J. (2016), “Common methods variance detection in business research”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 69 No. 8, pp. 3192-3198.

*Garnes, S. and Mathisen, G.E. (2014), “Organizational commitment of directors in collaborative tourist organizations: mediating effects of boardroom behavior”, Journal of Travel Research, Vol. 53 No. 4, pp. 448-461.

*Gnan, L. and Zattoni, A. (2009), “Board task performance in small firms”, in Huse, M. (Ed.), The Value Creating Board, Corporate Governance and Organizational Behaviour, Routledge, London, New York, NY, pp. 452-470.

Hedges, L.V. and Olkin, I. (1985), Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis, Academic Press, Orlando, FL.

Heemskerk, E.M., Heemskerk, K. and Wats, M.M. (2017), “Conflict in the boardroom: a participant observation study of supervisory board dynamics”, Journal of Management & Governance, Vol. 21 No. 1, pp. 233-263.

*Heemskerk, K., Heemskerk, E.M. and Wats, M. (2015), “Behavioral determinants of nonprofit board performance”, Nonprofit Management and Leadership, Vol. 25 No. 4, pp. 417-430.

Huse, M. (2009a), “Exploring methods and concepts in studies of board processes”, in Huse, M. (Ed.), The Value Creating Board, Corporate Governance and Organizational Behaviour, Routledge, London, New York, NY, pp. 221-233.

Huse, M. (2009b), “The ‘value creating board' surveys: a benchmark”, in Huse, M. (Ed.), The Value Creating Board, Corporate Governance and Organizational Behaviour, Routledge, London, New York, NY, pp. 367-383.

Huse, M. (2009c), “Building blocks in understanding behavioural perspectives of boards: developing a research stream”, in Huse, M. (Ed.), The Value Creating Board, Corporate Governance and Organizational Behaviour, Routledge, London, New York, NY, pp. 57-68.

Huse, M., Hoskisson, R., Zattoni, A. and Viganò, R. (2011), “New perspectives on board research: changing the research agenda”, Journal of Management & Governance, Vol. 15 No. 1, pp. 5-28.

Huybrechts, J., Voordeckers, W., D'Espallier, B., Lybaert, N. and Van Gils, A. (2016), “The board's demography – firm performance relationship revisited: a Bayesian approach”, Journal of Small Business Management, Vol. 54 No. 3, pp. 992-1007.

Jehn, K.A. (1995), “A multimethod examination of the benefits and detriments of intragroup conflict”, Administrative Science Quarterly, Vol. 40 No. 2, pp. 256-282.

Jehn, K.A. (2014), “Types of conflict: the history and future of conflict definitions and typologies”, in Ayoko, O.B., Ashkanasy, N.M. and Jehn, K.A. (Eds.), Handbook of Conflict Management Research, Edward Elgar Publishing, Cheltenham, Northampton, pp. 3-18.

Kumar, P. and Zattoni, A. (2017), “Board monitoring and effectiveness: antecedents and implications”, Corporate Governance: An International Review, Vol. 25 No. 2, pp. 76-77.

*McNulty, T., Florackis, C. and Ormrod, P. (2013), “Boards of directors and financial risk during the credit crisis”, Corporate Governance: An International Review, Vol. 21 No. 1, pp. 58-78.

Mande, B., Zuani Ishak, K.I. and Ammani, S. (2013), “Using structural equation modelling to explain board process and board performance in a developing economy”, International Journal of Global Business, Vol. 6 No. 1, pp. 58-80.

Marchewka, M. (2015), “Supervisory board group dynamics as a determinant of team and company effectiveness. empirical evidence from Poland”, Business and Public Administration Studies, Vol. 9 No. 1, pp. 17-23.

*Minichilli, A., Zattoni, A. and Zona, F. (2009), “Making boards effective: an empirical examination of board task performance”, British Journal of Management, Vol. 20 No. 1, pp. 55-74.

*Minichilli, A., Zattoni, A., Nielsen, S. and Huse, M. (2012), “Board task performance: an exploration of micro- and macro-level determinants of board effectiveness”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 33 No. 2, pp. 193-215.

*Mostert, I., Veltrop, D., van Veen-Dirks, P. and de Haan, J. (2015), “Pawn or vigilant watchdog? How board vigilance and board tenure moderate the effects of executive tenure on board functioning”, working paper 488, Netherlands Central Bank, Amsterdam, 21 December, available at www.dnb.nl/binaries/Working%20Paper%20488_tcm46-335346.pdf (accessed 17 August 2016).

*Msweli, P. and Singh, S. (2014), “An analysis of board attributes that contribute to decision quality in state-owned companies in South Africa”, Problems and Perspectives in Management, Vol. 12 No. 2, pp. 86-93.

Murphy, S.A. and McIntyre, M.L. (2007), “Board of director performance: a group dynamics perspective”, Corporate Governance: The International Journal of Business in Society, Vol. 7 No. 2, pp. 209-224.

*Namoga, M.O. (2011), Corporate governance and board performance: empirical evidence from pacific island countries, PhD thesis, James Cook University, Queensland, available at http://eprints.jcu.edu.au/18929 (accessed 2 February 2015).

Neill, D. and Dulewicz, V. (2010), “Inside the ‘black box’: the performance of boards of directors of unlisted companies”, Corporate Governance: The International Journal of Business in Society, Vol. 10 No. 3, pp. 293-306.

O'Neill, T.A., Allen, N.J. and Hastings, S.E. (2013), “Examining the ‘pros’ and ‘cons’ of team conflict: a team-level meta-analysis of task, relationship and process conflict”, Human Performance, Vol. 26 No. 3, pp. 236-260.

Ong, C.H. and Wan, D. (2008), “Three conceptual models of board role performance”, Corporate Governance: The International Journal of Business in Society, Vol. 8 No. 3, pp. 317-329.

Peterson, R.A. and Brown, S.P. (2005), “On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis”, The Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 90 No. 1, pp. 175-181.

Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Lee, J. and Podsakoff, N.P. (2003), “Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 88 No. 5, p. 879.

R Core Team (2016), R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.

*Ranasinghe, D. Mather, P.R. and Young, S. (2015), “Board processes, cognitive conflicts and financial reporting quality”, available at:http://ssrn.com/abstract=2618934 (accessed 17 August 2016).

*Scarborough, S., Haynie, J.J. and Shook, C. (2010), “Board composition, process, and activism: evidence within American firms”, Management & Marketing, Vol. 5 No. 1, pp. 3-18.

Schmidt, F.L. and Hunter, J.E. (2014), Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings, Sage publications, London.

Sellevoll, T., Huse, M. and Hansen, C. (2007), The value creating board: Results from the "Follow-Up Surveys" 2005/2006 in Norwegian firms, Norwegian School of Management BI, Oslo, Norway

Spector, P.E. (2006), “Method variance in organizational research truth or urban legend?”, Organizational Research Methods, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 221-232.

Sur, S. (2014), “Making sense of board effectiveness: a socio-cognitive perspective”, Corporate Governance: The International Journal of Business in Society, Vol. 14 No. 2, pp. 162-180.

Van Ees, H., Gabrielsson, J. and Huse, M. (2009), “Toward a behavioral theory of boards and corporate governance”, Corporate Governance: An International Review, Vol. 17 No. 3, pp. 307-319.

*Van Ees, H., Van der Laan, G. and Postma, T.J.B.M. (2008), “Effective board behavior in The Netherlands”, European Management Journal, Vol. 26 No. 2, pp. 84-93.

Viechtbauer, W. (2010), “Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package”, Journal of Statistical Software, Vol. 36 No. 3, pp. 1-48.

*Wan, D. and Ong, C.H. (2005), “Board structure, process and performance: evidence from public-listed companies in Singapore”, Corporate Governance: An International Review, Vol. 13 No. 2, pp. 277-290.

*Zattoni, A., Gnan, L. and Huse, M. (2015), “Does family involvement influence firm performance? Exploring the mediating effects of board processes and tasks”, Journal of Management, Vol. 41 No. 4, pp. 1214-1243.

Zona, F. (2015), “Board ownership and processes in family firms”, Small Business Economics, Vol. 44 No. 1, pp. 105-122.

*Zona, F. (2016), “CEO leadership and board decision processes in family-controlled firms: comparing family and non-family CEOs”, Small Business Economics, Vol. 47 No. 3, pp. 735-753.

*Zona, F. and Zattoni, A. (2007), “Beyond the black box of demography: board processes and task effectiveness within Italian firms”, Corporate Governance: An International Review, Vol. 15 No. 5, pp. 852-864.

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO) project no. 023.003 029.

Corresponding author

Klaas Heemskerk can be contacted at: k.b.t.heemskerk@uva.nl

About the author

Klaas Heemskerk is based at Amsterdam Institute for Social Science Research, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

Related articles