Bystander Apathy and Intervention in the Era of Social Media
The Emerald International Handbook of Technology-Facilitated Violence and Abuse
ISBN: 978-1-83982-849-2, eISBN: 978-1-83982-848-5
Publication date: 4 June 2021
Abstract
Bystander apathy has been a source of debate for decades. In the past half-century, psychologists developed theoretical frameworks to understand bystander activity, commonly referred to as bystander intervention models (BIMs). More recently, BIMs have been modified to facilitate initiatives to prevent various forms of online victimization. This chapter begins with a review of BIMs and recent applications of bystander intervention research to online environments. We also present several future directions for research along with applications for reducing technology-facilitated violence, including programming recommendations and theoretical development.
Keywords
Citation
Lytle, R.D., Bratton, T.M. and Hudson, H.K. (2021), "Bystander Apathy and Intervention in the Era of Social Media", Bailey, J., Flynn, A. and Henry, N. (Ed.) The Emerald International Handbook of Technology-Facilitated Violence and Abuse (Emerald Studies In Digital Crime, Technology and Social Harms), Emerald Publishing Limited, Leeds, pp. 711-728. https://doi.org/10.1108/978-1-83982-848-520211052
Publisher
:Emerald Publishing Limited
Copyright © 2021 Robert D. Lytle, Tabrina M. Bratton, and Heather K. Hudson. Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This chapter is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of these chapters (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode.
License
This chapter is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of these chapters (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode.
Introduction
If you have visited a metropolitan area, you have likely been present for an incident that could be considered an emergency. This might include walking past an unconscious person on the street one night, a shriek from a child as you walk past a park, or perhaps a heated argument between two adults in the neighboring room. When you perceive these types of stimuli in your environment, you engage in a cognitive process that helps to decide whether or not to act. This process includes a range of cognitive mechanisms wherein we interpret otherwise ambiguous information (e.g., Is the argument between the two adults becoming dangerous?), assess the situation (e.g., Can someone else assist the child in the park?), and consider how best to intervene (e.g., Do you wake the unconscious person and call for help?). Though we complete this process quickly, we remain partially paralyzed while resolving the conflict of witnessing a potential emergency.
Social scientists have generated a wealth of literature to explain the process driving bystander action in the presence of a possible emergency (Batson, van Lange, Ahmad, & Lishner, 2007). Much of this research has been built upon the bystander intervention model (BIM) proposed by Latané and Darley (1970) which situates the cognitive processes driving decisions to intervene in ambiguous emergencies within an unpredictable social environment. BIMs have informed applications to promote bystander intervention in emergencies, including violent situations, with some effect (Del Rey, Casas, & Ortega, 2016; Polanin, Espelage, & Pigott, 2012).
Despite this success, society has experienced significant changes in both cognitive processing and environments following the rise of social media. This chapter begins by briefly reviewing the literature on bystander apathy and BIMs, with a special emphasis on developments in bystander theory in light of recent technological advancements. The remainder of the chapter is dedicated to proposed directions for developing theory, methodological considerations for future research, and applying BIMs to understanding and reducing technology-facilitated violence and abuse (TFVA).
The Bystander Intervention Model
In 1968, John Darley and Bibb Latané published a seminal work on an emerging psychological concept referred to as bystander apathy. Bystander apathy refers to the phenomenon wherein otherwise moral persons can observe an emergency (e.g., accident, crime) without coming to the aid of those affected by it. Although incidents of bystander apathy were not novel at the time, social commentary grew in the wake of the murder of Kitty Genovese in 1964 (Manning, Levins, & Collins, 2007). Genovese was reportedly murdered while bystanders in the nearby apartments, all aware of the attack, did little to intervene on her behalf. Explanations centered around the moral ambivalence or decay of society at the time. Latané and Darley (1970), however, hypothesized that bystander apathy might emerge from the internal conflict one experiences when confronted with an emergency. In effect, they argued that bystander apathy did not necessarily imply a moral deficiency in the passive bystander; rather, they argued nonintervention could be expected when the situation surrounding the emergency favored nonintervention (Darley & Latané, 1968). After several classic experiments, Latané and Darley (1970) presented their BIM to explain bystander decisions about whether or not to intervene in emergencies.
Latané and Darley's (1970) model holds that people experience internal conflict when observing an emergency, regardless of their desire to assist or intervene in the danger. The BIM consists of five steps that influence whether or not a person will intervene. The first step is to witness the incident. Once a potential emergency has been witnessed, the second step requires the bystander to define the situation as an emergency. If the danger to the bystander or victim is unclear, the bystander may follow others' lead in recognizing whether the ambiguous situation (e.g., smoke, loud sounds) demands a response (Clark & Word, 1972; Darley & Latané, 1968; Latané & Darley, 1970). This is referred to as audience inhibition or pluralistic ignorance (Latané & Nida, 1981). This psychological process is so impactful that, even when the situation becomes more clearly threatening, the bystander will remain calm if others witnessing the situation do not react to the threat (Darley & Latané, 1968).
If the bystander defines the situation as an emergency, they then look to the environment for justification to avoid the risk often present in an intervention (e.g., injury, embarrassment) (Latané & Darley, 1970). If a bystander is alone with the victim of an emergency, the responsibility of intervening on the victim's behalf is placed entirely on the bystander. As such, the cost of intervention may not outweigh the moral obligation they feel. However, as the number of other persons capable of intervening increases, the bystander can mentally share the responsibility for providing aid to a victim, which is known as diffusion of responsibility (Darley & Latané, 1968; Garcia, Weaver, Moskowitz, & Darley, 2002). As a result, the moral obligation felt by the bystander is attenuated and may not overcome the costs associated with intervention (Darley & Latané, 1968). Scholars often refer to nonintervention facilitated by bystander group size as the “bystander effect” (Levine, Philpot, & Kovalenko, 2020).
Although the BIM has enjoyed the theoretical spotlight for understanding helping behaviors, recent developments have revealed conditions to the initial framework (Fischer et al., 2011). One such development in bystander intervention research has centered around the nature of the emergency. Whereas classic bystander effect studies observed inaction in response to emergencies that posed little to no immediate danger to the participant (Levine & Darley, 1968), more recent research has proposed that dangerous or violent emergencies may be less likely to result in bystander inaction; a phenomenon commonly referred to as positive bystander effects (Fischer et al., 2011; Fischer, Greitemeyer, Pollozek, & Frey, 2006). Another relevant development in the bystander intervention literature relevant to understanding online environments involves the group composition of persons surrounding the bystander during the emergency (referred to here as witnesses). Based on meta-analyses of bystander intervention research, helping behavior is more likely to occur when a bystander is surrounded by witnesses who are known to one another (Rutkowski, Gruder, & Romer, 1983), share membership in a common social or demographic group (Levine & Crowther, 2008; Levine, Prosser, Evans, & Reicher, 2005), or are simply active observers (Fischer & Greitemeyer, 2013). Relatedly, bystanders who know the victim are also more likely to intervene in an emergency regardless of the size (or composition) of fellow witnesses to the emergency (Palmer, Nicksa, & McMahon, 2018). In this regard, bystander intervention may be similar in online environments. That is, helping behavior is likely dependent on the online relationship between the witness and victim.
Nonetheless, the BIM has demonstrated a moderately weak effect in explaining bystander inaction (Fischer et al., 2011). The utility of this theory can also be observed through its application to various forms of violence reduction and intervention programs, including bullying (Polanin et al., 2012) and campus sexual assault (Banyard, Moynihan, Cares, & Warner, 2014). 1 It has, however, emerged as a helpful tool to combat non-reporting and inaction from persons who observe an emergency, especially incidents that may traditionally have been viewed as “private,” such as domestic violence (Levine et al., 2020; McMahon & Banyard, 2012; Palmer et al., 2018).
Bystander Intervention Models in Online Environments
Social interaction has changed significantly since Latané and Darley's (1970) seminal work. In particular, the rise of technological advancements in communication, such as social media, has changed societal norms, behaviors, and social environments. There are numerous benefits to social media, such as widening one's social networks and providing an outlet for communication and knowledge generation. However, the ubiquitous nature of social media today also has the potential to facilitate antisocial online behavior (e.g., Hayes, 2019; Southworth, Finn, Dawson, Fraser, & Tucker, 2007; Woodlock, 2017), including the targeting of minority populations such as racial minorities and members of the LGBTQI communities. Antisocial and cybercrime behaviors like online harassment can have detrimental impacts on the social, mental, and physical wellness of victims (Bates, 2017; Henry et al., 2020; Powell, Scott, Henry, & Flynn, 2020; Powell, Henry, & Flynn, 2018; Tennant, Demaray, Coyle, & Malecki, 2015; Widgerson & Lynch, 2013). These consequences not only lead to stigmatization, they may contribute to psychological distress (Wright, 2016) and suicide (Bauman, Toomey, & Walker, 2013; Sullivan, Annest, Simon, Luo, & Dahlberg, 2015).
Although the emergence of social media has undoubtedly changed the ways and environment in which we communicate with each other, the classic bystander effect may continue to inhibit bystanders from intervening online (Dempsey, Sulkowski, Nichols, & Storch, 2009; Olenik-Shemesh, Heiman, & Eden, 2017; Sticca & Perren, 2013). One possible source of bystander inaction in online environments may be ambiguity in cybervictimization. Cybervictimization, along with the online social environment they occur within, is relatively new. As a result, the norms and values for observing and responding to status updates in social media continue to evolve, which may create uncertainty for witnesses regarding what is and is not acceptable behavior online (Banyard, Plante, & Moynihan, 2005). In much the same way that perpetrators can benefit from anonymity in online environments, bystanders of online victimization may be able to avoid the cognitive conflict that often accompanies witnessing a potential emergency (Dempsey et al., 2009). Intervention may be particularly unlikely when the bystander does not know the victim well (Palmer et al., 2018). Finally, the lack of physical presence can make it complicated for bystanders to intervene in the emergency. This is especially the case when considering a lack of parental socialization toward online defending behavior reported by many adolescents (DeSmet et al., 2014). Witnesses inclined to physically intervene in offline situations (e.g., putting their body in front of a victim) may be uncertain about the best way to intervene in online environments. Despite these differences, however, it appears BIMs apply in both settings (Hayes, 2019).
Much of the research on bystander intervention in online environments has centered around cyberbullying, sexual violence, and intimate partner violence (IPV; Hayes, 2019). This research has generally found that bystander helping behavior, often taking the form of defending the targeted person, can be predicted by a bystander's social group, attitudes, and situational influences. More specifically, research has found that online bystander intervention is most likely to occur when the bystander is female (Allison & Bussey, 2017; Lambe, Della Cioppa, Hong, & Craig, 2019), older (Hayes, 2019; Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2017), and where they enjoy social support or popularity (Lambe et al., 2019; Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2017). Bystanders are less likely to intervene when they hold negative views of the victim (Hayes, 2019) or decline to speak up against prejudice (Moisuc, Brauer, Fonseca, Chaurand, & Greitemeyer, 2018). Finally, the norms and values of a bystander's environment, including those learned from family (DeSmet et al., 2014; Lambe et al., 2019) or school (Allison & Bussey, 2016, 2017), influence bystander intention to defend, as well as actual defending behaviors in cyberbullying events.
Interestingly, though, the ubiquity of social media might also provide a protective effect for victims. For example, victims of cybercrime and online harassment may have more social support than they have offline. To the extent that others might engage in the BIM in online environments, social media may contribute to the prevention of online victimization (e.g., positive bystander effect), as well as curbing the adverse effects of such victimization. Indeed, some scholars have found evidence of a positive bystander effect in online settings (Armstrong & Mahone, 2017). For example, online intervention is more likely, regardless of group size, when witnesses perceive the emergency as being more clearly dangerous or there is a relationship between the bystander and witnesses or victim. This can occur when online bystanders are aware of the dangers associated with online victimization (Armstrong & Mahone, 2017) or share a common identity or group identity (DeSmet et al., 2016; Lambe et al., 2019).
Considerations for Studying Bystander Intervention in Social Media
A variety of complexities arise when applying offline BIMs to online incidents. As BIMs rely heavily on the environment to explain bystander action, it follows that the theoretical pathways used to explain responses to emergencies may differ based on whether they occur offline or online. In this section, we review some of the ways that traditional BIMs may need further examination in research before application to online social environments.
The first step of BIMs is that one must observe the incident (Latané & Darley, 1970). In online settings, especially through social media, it may be difficult to determine when bystanders become aware of potential emergencies (Dillon & Bushman, 2015; McMahon & Banyard, 2012). For example, many social networking sites (SNSs) have a “wall” of posts where comments from followers are brought to the user's attention. SNS walls can be a public “place” used to bully, embarrass, and shame or defend, support, and praise other SNS users. Because of the breadth of information one sees on social media, online bystanders might become aware of incidents days after it occurred (McMahon, Palmer, Banyard, Murphy, & Gidycz, 2017), thereby removing the immediacy of observing the incident from the classic BIM. In light of the asynchronous nature of the online environment, this may be a key consideration for future research.
The second step of the BIM is that the bystander must define the incident as an emergency (Latané & Darley, 1970). As outlined earlier, this process can be influenced by the reactions of others present at the incident (Darley & Latané, 1968). BIMs that apply in the online context may need additional research to clarify the cognitive processes by which bystanders evaluate danger online, including content and other signals for help (Schachter, Greenberg, & Juvonen, 2016).
The keystone of traditional BIMs has been the influence of bystander group size on intervention (Latané & Darley, 1970). Presently, much of the research into online bystander intervention has focused on how diffusion of responsibility affects bystander responses to online emergencies, with a general conclusion being that the presence of online bystanders discourages bystander action (Dempsey et al., 2009; Hayes, 2019; Sticca & Perren, 2013). Although these studies provide an excellent starting point for online bystander intervention research, there remain several important questions for understanding bystander effects online. For example, how do online bystanders become aware of the presence of other bystanders? The anonymity of many online social environments may affect a person's ability to diffuse responsibility if they are not certain others are available to assist the victim. Conversely, anonymity in social media may allow the bystander to rationalize inaction by assuming others are present and observing the same emergency, therefore someone else may step in. Larger bystander group size has been shown to discourage intervention by simply having bystanders think about the presence of other witnesses (Garcia et al., 2002). Therefore, it may be easy for online bystanders to assume the presence of other witnesses regardless of ambiguous evidence.
In addition to applying the classic bystander effect to harassment and violence, research into online bystander intervention should also consider the possibility of positive bystander effects (Fischer et al., 2006). When the victim is in clear danger, such as violence, a larger number of witnesses actually facilitates bystander intervention, regardless of the assumed personal cost to bystanders. This finding has been attributed to both the less ambiguous nature of the threat to a victim in violent encounters (Piliavin, Rodin, & Piliavin, 1969) and the improved chance of successful intervention given the support of other bystanders (Fischer et al., 2011). This makes recognizing danger in online incidents particularly relevant when designing strategies to prevent cybervictimization.
Applications to Technology-Facilitated Violence and Abuse
BIMs can inform the public about TFVA, educate users about how to identify online problems, and provide recommendations for intervention (Moynihan et al., 2015). Evaluations of school-based BIM programs have proved successful in preventing violence in educational settings, especially among high school (Coker, Bush, Brancato, Clear, & Recktenwald, 2019; Polanin et al., 2012) and college students (Banyard, Moynihan, & Plate, 2007). Although most school-based bystander intervention programs do not use all five stages of the BIM, Nickerson, Aloe, Livingston, & Feeley (2014) suggested relying more on BIM to develop school-based, online bystander programs that explicitly teach the five steps of BIMs to decrease online bystander apathy. BIM-driven programs could facilitate the identification of TFVA, interpretation of TFVA as situations requiring intervention, assumption of responsibility for defending the victim, and learning the skills necessary to intervene safely.
Currently, few online bystander intervention programs target TFVA. The BIM can inform strategies to reduce bystander apathy and redirect passive attitudes into acceptable online behavior. Bystander intervention can reduce TFVA by interfering in violent situations before they escalate. Recent research on online bystanders have identified factors that increase the likelihood for intervention such as gender, age, perceived danger, familiarity with the victim, and attitudes (Allison & Bussey, 2016, 2017; Bastiaensens et al., 2014; Hayes, 2019; Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2017). Unfortunately, many of these factors are difficult to implement in programming (i.e., knowing victim, social popularity, and classroom). Furthermore, existing bystander intervention programming, especially with relation to sexual assault prevention, may be difficult to generalize to cases beyond cisgendered, heterosexual male offenders, and female victims (Dunn, Bailey, & Msosa, 2020). Based on existing research, then, intervention training should focus on the factors that can be changed through programming, like helping attitudes and the perceived danger of emergency.
To apply the first step of BIM (Latané & Darley, 1970), observation of the incident can be promoted by raising awareness to TFVA behaviors and warning signs. BIMs could also teach users to identify alarming content and TFVA as an emergency (Step 2). A key decision point to address in online bystander intervention programming will likely be the identification of the situation as an emergency. Recent research has suggested that many forms of TFVA reviewed in this chapter (and beyond), may be perceived by both the offender and witnesses as attempts at humor or “having fun” gone awry (Retter & Shariff, 2012; Shariff & DeMartini, 2016; Shariff, Wiseman, & Crestohl, 2012). Although this research has focused primarily on the motivation of the offender, this finding may also apply to bystander assessments of situations as an emergency. If bystanders believe that some event is a poorly executed joke, or they can quickly rationalize it as such, they can define the situation as a non-emergency and avoid responsibility for action. Multiple strategies may be needed to encourage bystanders to accept responsibility for intervention (Step 3). These strategies should promote social norms that encourage empathy for victims of TFVA (Nickerson et al., 2014), as well as increase self-efficacy for intervention among bystanders (Pöyhönen, Juvonen, & Salmivalli, 2012). Alternatively, online bystander intervention programs should consider how internet users perceive the presence of others to combat diffusion of responsibility and pluralistic ignorance, and identify ways to address these challenges (Darley & Latané, 1968; Latané & Darley, 1970).
Along with recognizing and accepting responsibility for TFVA incidents, BIM holds that bystanders must have a plan for action (Step 4) and be able to intervene (Step 5; Latané & Darley, 1970). Indeed, bystanders who are better trained in how to respond may be better prepared and therefore, more likely to assist in online emergencies effectively and safely (Coker et al., 2011). These strategies should focus on building knowledge and skills regarding how best to act. Although social media may promote opportunities for intervention (such as flagging or reporting TFVA), some aspects of social media like anonymity appear to hinder bystander intervention (Dempsey et al., 2009; Sticca & Perren, 2013). Therefore, programming should present tangible ways to intervene in specific types of TFVA.
Application #1: Technology-Facilitated Suicidality
Suicidality has long garnered attention, but only recently have online presentations of suicidality been considered a path to prevention. Some research suggests that adolescents who have suicidal ideations or engage in self-harm may share their feelings and practices on SNSs (Luxton, June, & Fairall, 2012; Marchant et al., 2017). Moreover, suicidality has been linked to social media use (Mok, Jorm, & Pirkis, 2015) and cybervictimization (Hinduja & Patchin, 2010). Witnesses to suicidal or self-harming posts can escalate self-harm through demeaning comments. As such, bystanders to online suicidal ideation and self-harm represent an opportunity to reduce and prevent suicidality. To observe and intervene in suicidal threats or ideations online, internet users should be trained to identify direct and indirect warning signs (Marchant et al., 2017). For example, programs might include ways to teach users to identify risky hashtags (e.g., #cutting), take them seriously, and respond appropriately.
To accomplish raising the perceived seriousness of online suicidal ideation, programs should incorporate peers into awareness campaigns. For example, peer-facilitators could confront the acceptance of jokes about suicide and rally others to treat all threats seriously. Peer-led bystander intervention exposure has previously been implemented with success in student populations, especially when many students were trained to be peer leaders (Pickering et al., 2018). Even if online suicidal threats are perceived as worthy of intervention, though, BIMs predict that bystanders will be less likely to respond if one can diffuse responsibility with other witnesses (Latané & Darley, 1970). To encourage internet users to accept responsibility, programs can focus on activities that promote empathy for suicidal persons and self-efficacy in intervention, both of which have been shown to discourage diffusion of responsibility online (Nickerson et al., 2014; Pöyhönen et al., 2012).
Even if the witness identifies the situation as an emergency, however, the stress and anxiety produced in individuals intervening in suicidality may create a specific obstacle to bystander action for this form of TFVA (Ting, Jacobson, & Sanders, 2011). In offline settings, the classic bystander apathy effect reverses so that witnesses become less likely to intervene when alone if the emergency poses a danger to the witness (Fischer & Greitemeyer, 2013). Therefore, BIMs intended to address technology-facilitated suicidality might also need to consider ways to help potential interveners process and cope with such anxiety. For example, social media campaigns can educate their users on the importance of intervention despite one's discomfort. In addition, programs to encourage bystander intervention in online environments can include services for interveners should their experience prove to be traumatic.
Peer-facilitators can also role play scenarios to help internet users develop strategies for effective intervention, which can empower bystanders in the face of an emergency. Effective plans for intervention can develop through skills-training, access to effective resources, and education on what works and what worsens the situation. Training programs should guide internet users through scenarios to explore direct and indirect options for intervening. Direct interventions for online suicidality could include approaches to talking to persons threatening self-harm online. Alternatively, bystanders may offer support and resources intended to help suicidal persons (e.g., suicide prevention phone lines or licensed counselors). Indirect measures would include strategies for bringing attention to the suicidal content, such as contacting emergency services in an immediate crisis or reporting alarming content on social media platforms. The benefits for bystander prevention in online suicidality are cumulative in the sense that intervention from one bystander may encourage others to act as well (Darley & Latané, 1968; Latané & Nida, 1981).
Application #2: Image-Based Sexual Abuse
Another source of TFVA that has grown from the ubiquity of social media has been image-based sexual abuse (IBSA). IBSA refers to dissemination of sexualized materials without the subject's consent (Bond & Tyrrell, 2018; Henry & Flynn, 2020). Although still early in its research development, scholars have noted significant health consequences for IBSA that range from symptoms of psychological distress to suicidality (Bates, 2017; Henry, McGlynn, Flynn, Johnson, Powell, & Scott, 2020; Powell et al., 2020; Powell et al., 2018). Despite recognition of the consequences of IBSA for victims, policymakers and police have reported difficulties designing effective responses to IBSA (Bond & Tyrrell, 2018; Henry et al., 2020; Henry, Flynn, & Powell, 2018). This difficulty stems from ambiguity in the definition of IBSA, especially in determining what material should be considered illicit, nonconsensual, and sexual (Bond & Tyrrell, 2018; Flynn & Henry, 2019; Walker & Sleath, 2017).
In light of these limitations, bystander intervention may be useful in discouraging IBSA. IBSA grows in online environments, wherein bystanders not only ignore the victimization but also reward its production (Henry & Flynn, 2020; Walker & Sleath, 2017). In addition, bystanders might assume the sexual material was published consensually and may not accurately recognize it as a form of victimization. Furthermore, if bystanders perceive the IBSA as a “joke” or venting frustration (Shariff & DeMartini, 2016), the situation may be defined as a non-emergency. Consequently, intervention would be perceived to be unnecessary. To the degree that bystander apathy contributes to the proliferation of IBSA, then, we may be able to quell its use, support police efforts to take action, and raise public awareness to the methods and consequences of this form of victimization.
Public education campaigns can help internet users and website administrators recognize and value curbing nonconsensual sexual materials, which could increase intervention on the victim's behalf. Furthermore, BIM programs can train online bystanders on what actions may be best suited to dissuade the spread of IBSA (Powell et al., 2020). For example, the Australian Federal Government has tasked the Office of the eSafety Commissioner with responsibility for addressing IBSA. One such example is that the Office has created a website which provides the public with information on online victimization and services to victims of cybercrime and IBSA, including a portal where victims can report their victimization experiences. 2 As victimization in cyberspace does not take place in a single geography, governments should consider direct action through similar approaches to this initiative implemented in Australia. In addition, social media can publicize “warning signs” and appropriate actions victims and bystanders can use to report potential IBSA to administrators or police. By training bystanders to recognize IBSA, define it as a threat to the victim, take responsibility for action, and make a plan to intervene, BIM programs may offer utility to reduction efforts.
Application #3: Cyberbullying and Online Harassment
BIMs are increasingly common in cyberbullying prevention (Allison & Bussey, 2016). Bullying refers to intentionally aggressive and repetitive acts targeting persons less able to defend themselves (Smith et al., 2008). Cyberbullying involves bullying that occurs online. The prevalence of bystanders in cyberbullying incidents (DeSmet et al., 2016) combined with how easily bystanders might unintentionally support cyberbullies for example, liking a harmful post, sharing a humiliating image (Barlinksa, Szuster, & Winiewski, 2013) make bystander behavior in cyberbullying incidents particularly important. Some scholars have offered recommendations for promoting bystander intervention in cyberbullying contexts, such as defending or comforting the victim (DeSmet et al., 2016). This is especially possible when bystanders empathize with victims and are inclined toward positive bystander behavior (DeSmet et al., 2016).
There have been a number of programs designed to promote bystander intervention in cyberbullying situations (DeSmet et al., 2018). Many of these programs have emphasized the first two stages of the BIM recognizing the situation and defining it as an emergency (Del Rey et al., 2016; DeSmet et al., 2018). More specifically, cyberbullying intervention programs often center around raising participant attention to the signals and dangers of cyberbullying. Furthermore, these programs develop participant empathy toward cyberbullying victims (Allison & Bussey, 2016, 2017).
A central component to BIM programming should be to foster a sense of responsibility for bystanders (DeSmet et al., 2016). This might be accomplished through demonstrations of the bystander effect or role playing scenarios. A more sustained target for promoting bystander responsibility, though, might be social norms regarding internet etiquette and social support. Training teenagers to serve as “active bystanders” in social media environments may influence technological norms for generations. Furthermore, active bystanders can help publicize cyberbullying incidents, rally support to victims, and bring repeated incidents to the attention of authorities (e.g., parents or police).
BIMs may also offer guidance for cyberbullying programs regarding bystander plans for taking action. BIM programs might engage participants in critical thinking exercises to explore potential action plans, including direct, indirect, and distracting interventions. Direct intervention includes actions to help the victim that insert the bystander directly into the cyberbullying incident. For example, a bystander might directly intervene through confrontation, replying with an anti-bullying emoji, or documenting the incident. Indirect intervention would involve bystander actions that help the victim without getting the bystander involved in the incident, such as reporting and flagging content. Bystanders might also use distraction to intervene on the victim's behalf, which serves to “change the topic” away from the cyberbullying content toward something more positive about the victim. Finally, intervention in cyberbullying incidents can also be promoted using BIMs by encouraging participants to develop a plan of action so they are not only prepared to act, but also feel less anxiety about intervention.
Using Bystander Intervention Models to Address Technology-Facilitated Violence and Abuse
BIMs can be used to reduce bystander apathy, encourage decisions to intervene in emergencies, and perhaps even prevent TFVA by deterring violence. The evolving environment surrounding social media and online social interactions makes it difficult to anticipate changes in harmful online activities. However, if more internet users are trained to be active bystanders who recognize the potential danger that various forms of TFVA pose to victims, as well as the need for bystanders to reject the temptation to share responsibility for action, there will be a strong foundation for prevention and intervention efforts.
A goal for researchers moving forward, then, should be to generate “best practices” for online bystander intervention programs. These best practices should build on research in online and offline bystander intervention, including positive bystander intervention effects and perpetrator-victim-bystander relationships. Based on the current state of the literature, bystander intervention programs can facilitate bystander recognition of potentially dangerous situations by presenting, in a publicly translatable manner, changes in the nature, and processes in TFVA. Furthermore, programs should help users to clearly identify the warning signs of online harassment and violence. It is also important for programs to establish the seriousness of TFVA in online environments through public education initiatives, such as public service announcements and awareness campaigns. These efforts may involve developing empathy toward victims of various forms of TFVA, as empathy remains a consistent predictor of positive bystander action in online settings (e.g., DeSmet et al., 2016).
Online bystander intervention programs should also incorporate strategies for discouraging diffusion of responsibility in bystanders witnessing online harassment or violence. Existing research has suggested that online bystanders are susceptible to the same “bystander effect” that has been observed in offline settings. Indeed, some studies have indicated an increased impact of group size on bystander apathy in online situations (Fischer & Greitemeyer, 2013; Levine & Crowther, 2008). Key obstacles to encouraging bystanders to accept responsibility include the anonymity of online witnesses to harassment and violence (Brody & Vangelisti, 2015), ambiguity in the danger posed to the victim in online settings (Schachter et al., 2016), and social norms regulating defending bullying victims (Barlinksa et al., 2013). Finally, BIMs applied to social media would encourage public discourse and education about different options for intervention, highlighting (a) the variety of interventions that can fit the bystander's “intervention style” and (b) the benefit that intervention offers to the victim.
Although these strategies provide a useful first step in promoting online bystander intervention, there remains a significant body of research required to understand the processes by which online bystanders decide to act. One area of research that has received attention in BIMs cyberbullying programming includes positive bystander effects (Fischer & Greitemeyer, 2013; Levine & Crowther, 2008). When bystanders are aware of the danger posed to the victim in the emergency, not only are they more likely to personally intervene but also larger group sizes can serve as support for the bystander to intervene (Fischer et al., 2011; Levine et al., 2005). Processes like these, by which bystander action is heightened offline, may offer useful insight into effective strategies for encouraging bystander intervention online. Furthermore, although their effects will be less immediate, initiatives to change online etiquette and social norms can provide a more sustained contribution to bystander intervention. Indeed, by promoting the defense of persons targeted in TFVA, online bystanders may be primed to confront perpetrators and support victims.
There has also been a noteworthy recent call for BIM research to incorporate an intersectional approach (Dunn et al., 2020). Currently, much of the literature has been based on existing theoretical models that treat bystander apathy as monolithically applied across marginalized groups (e.g., race, ethnicity, gender expression, sexuality, age, ability, etc.; Bang, Kerrick, & Wuthrich, 2017). This unrealistic treatment may pose several threats to the validity of BIMs, however. Many of the current theoretical models have been developed using cisgendered, heterosexual, White populations, often situated with a male offender and female victim (Dunn et al., 2020). Therefore, these models may have limited generalizability for other marginalized groups and offender/victim combinations. For example, bystanders who are persons of color may experience a greater risk for intervening in offenses committed by White men (Wooten, 2017). As scholars work to develop BIM-driven research in online settings, then, there should be efforts made to use samples and programming tailored to such an intersectional approach.
Conclusion
We began this chapter with hypothetical scenarios that would likely be relevant to anyone who has traveled for work or leisure. In the era of social media, though, it may even be more likely for you to have noticed that an active Facebook friend has suddenly desisted from engaging with friends. Or, you may have seen comments on Twitter wherein a friend has increasingly referenced self-harm in online interactions. Although social media can bring people with a common purpose together, it can also serve as a virtual battleground, wherein arguments can evolve into threats and, occasionally, physical or psychological damage. The value of bystander intervention remains as important as it was when Kitty Genovese was brutally attacked in 1964, particularly in the online context. With our attention turned toward often ambiguous online emergencies, then, researchers should not allow the responsibility for further development of BIMs and design for intervention programs to diffuse.
Notes
Levine et al. (2020) have recently offered a series of limitations that should be considered when building violence reduction programs out of classic BIM. These recommendations center around programs that apply Latané and Darley's (1970) original model without incorporating recent developments in the research.
This website can be found at https://www.esafety.gov.au/about-us/research/image-based-abuse.
References
Allison and Bussey, 2016 Allison, K. R. , & Bussey, K. (2016). Cyber-by standing in context: A review of the literature on witnesses' responses to cyberbullying. Children and Youth Services Review, 65, 183–194.
Allison and Bussey, 2017 Allison, K. R. , & Bussey, K. (2017). Individual and collective moral influences on intervention in cyberbullying. Computers in Human Behavior, 74, 7–15.
Armstrong and Mahone, 2017 Armstrong, C. L. , & Mahone, J. (2017). “It's on us”: The role of social media and rape culture in individual willingness to mobilize against sexual assault. Mass Communication and Society, 20, 92–115.
Bang et al., 2017 Bang, A. , Kerrick, A. , & Wuthrich, C. K. (2017). Examining bystander intervention in the wake of #BlackLivesMatter and #TransLivesMatter. In S. C. Wooten & R. W. Mitchell (Eds.), Preventing sexual violence on campus: Challenging traditional approaches through program innovation (pp. 65–80). New York, NY: Routledge.
Banyard et al., 2014 Banyard, V. L. , Moynihan, M. M. , Cares, A. C. , & Warner, R. (2014). How do we know if it works? Measuring outcomes in bystander-focused abuse prevention on campuses. Psychology of Violence, 4(1), 101–115.
Banyard et al., 2007 Banyard, V. L. , Moynihan, M. M. , & Plante, E. G. (2007). Sexual violence prevention through bystander education: An experimental evaluation. Journal of Community Psychology, 35, 463–481.
Banyard et al., 2005 Banyard, V. L. , Plante, E. G. , & Moynihan, M. M. (2005). Rape prevention through bystander education: Bringing a broader community perspective to sexual violence prevention. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. Retrieved from https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdf-files1/nij/grants/208701.pdf
Barlinksa et al., 2013 Barlinksa, J. , Szuster, A. , & Winiewski, M. (2013). Cyberbullying among adolescent bystanders: Role of the communication medium, form of violence, and empathy. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 23, 37–51.
Bastiaensens et al., 2014 Bastiaensens, S. , Vanderbosch, H. , Poels, K. , Van Cleemput, K. , DeSmet, A. , & Bourdeadhuij, I. (2014). Cyberbullying on social network sites: An experimental study into bystanders' behavioural intentions to help the victim or reinforce the bully. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 259–271.
Bates, 2017 Bates, S. (2017). Revenge porn and mental health: A qualitative analysis of the mental health effects of revenge porn on female survivors. Feminist Criminology, 12(1), 22–42.
Batson et al., 2007 Batson, C. D. , van Lange, P. A. M. , Ahmad, N. , & Lishner, D. A. (2007). Altruism and helping behavior. In M. A. Hogg & J. Cooper (Eds.), The Sage handbook of social psychology: Concise student edition (pp. 241–258). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Bauman et al., 2013 Bauman, S. , Toomey, R. B. , & Walker, J. L. (2013). Associations among bullying, cyberbullying, and suicide in high school students. Journal of Adolescence, 36, 341–350.
Bond and Tyrrell, 2018 Bond, E. , & Tyrrell, K. (2018). Understanding revenge pornography: A national survey of police officers and staff in England and Wales. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. doi:10.1177/0886260518760011
Brody and Vangelisti, 2015 Brody, N. , & Vangelisti, A. L. (2015). Bystander intervention in cyberbullying. Communication Monographs, 83(1), 94–116.
Clark and Word, 1972 Clark, R. D. , & Word, L. E. (1972). Why don't bystanders help? Because of ambiguity?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 392–400.
Coker et al., 2019 Coker, A. L. , Bush, H. N. , Brancato, C. J. , Clear, E. R. , & Recktenwald, E. A. (2019). Bystander program effectiveness to reduce violence acceptance: RCT in high schools. Journal of Family Violence, 34(3), 153–164.
Coker et al., 2011 Coker, A. L. , Cook-Craig, P. G. , Williams, C. M. , Fisher, B. S. , Clear, E. R. , Garcia, L. S. , & Hegge, L. M. (2011). Evaluation of green dot: An active bystander intervention to reduce sexual violence on college campuses. Violence Against Women, 17(6), 777–796.
Darley and Latané , 1968 Darley, J. M. , & Latané, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8(4), 377–383.
Del Rey et al., 2016 Del Rey, R. , Casas, J. A. , & Ortega, R. (2016). Impact of the ConRed program on different cyberbullying roles. Aggressive Behavior, 42, 123–135.
Dempsey et al., 2009 Dempsey, A. G. , Sulkowski, M. L. , Nichols, R. , & Storch, E. A. (2009). Differences between peer victimization in cyber and physical settings and associated psychosocial adjustment in early adolescence. Psychology in the Schools, 46, 962–972.
DeSmet et al., 2016 DeSmet, A. , Bastiaensens, S. , van Cleemput, K. , Poels, K. , Vandebosch, H. , Cardon, G. , & de Bourdeaudhuij, I. (2016). Deciding whether to look after them, to like it, or leave it: A multidimensional analysis of predictors of positive and negative bystander behavior in cyberbullying among adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 57, 398–415.
DeSmet et al., 2018 DeSmet, A. , Bastiaensens, S. , van Cleemput, K. , Poels, K. , Vandebosch, H. , Deboutte, G. , … de Bourdeaudhuij, I. (2018). The efficacy of Friendly Attac serious digital game to promote prosocial bystander behavior in cyberbullying among young adolescents: A cluster-randomized controlled trial. Computers in Human Behavior, 78, 336–347.
DeSmet et al., 2014 DeSmet, A. , Veldeman, C. , Poels, K. , Bastiaensens, S. , van Cleemput, K. , Vandebosch, H. , & de Bourdeaudhuij, I. (2014). Determinants of self-reported bystander behavior in cyberbullying incidents amongst adolescents. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17(4), 207–215.
Dillon and Bushman, 2015 Dillon, K. P. , & Bushman, B. J. (2015). Unresponsive or un-noticed? Cyberbystander intervention in an experimental cyberbullying context. Computers in Human Behavior, 45, 144–150.
Dunn et al., 2020 Dunn, S. , Bailey, J. , & Msosa, Y. (2020). Stand by me: Viewing bystander intervention programming through an intersectional lens. In D. Crocker , J. Minaker , & A. Neuland (Eds.), Violence interrupted: Confronting sexual violence on university campuses (pp. 117–140). Montreal, QC: McGill-Queen’s University Press.
Fischer and Greitemeyer, 2013 Fischer, P. , & Greitemeyer, T. (2013). The positive bystander effect: Passive bystanders increase helping in situations with high expected negative consequences for the helper. Journal of Social Psychology, 153(1), 1–5.
Fischer et al., 2006 Fischer, P. , Greitemeyer, T. , Pollozek, F. , & Frey, D. (2006). The unresponsive bystander: Are bystanders more responsive in dangerous emergencies?. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 267–278.
Fischer et al., 2011 Fischer, P. , Krueger, J. I. , Greitemeyer, T. , Vogrincic, C. , Kastenmüller, A. , Frey, D. , … Kainbacher, M. (2011). The bystander-effect: A meta-analytic review on bystander intervention in dangerous and non-dangerous emergencies. Psychological Bulletin, 137(4), 517–537.
Flynn and Henry, 2019 Flynn, A. , & Henry, N. (2019). Image-based sexual abuse: An Australian reflection. Women and Criminal Justice, online first. doi:10.1080/08974554.2019.1646190
Garcia et al., 2002 Garcia, S. M. , Weaver, K. , Moskowitz, G. B. , & Darley, J. M. (2002). Crowded minds: The implicit bystander effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(4), 843–853.
Hayes, 2019 Hayes, B. E. (2019). Bystander intervention to abusive behavior on social networking websites. Violence Against Women, 25(4), 463–484.
Henry and Flynn, 2020 Henry, N. , & Flynn, A. (2020). Image-based sexual abuse: Online distribution channels and illicit communities of support. Violence Against Women, 26(16), 1932–1955.
Henry et al., 2018 Henry, N. , Flynn, A. , & Powell, A. (2018). Policing image-based sexual abuse: Stakeholder perspectives. Police Practice and Research: An International Journal, 19(6), 565–581.
Henry et al., 2020 Henry, N. , McGlynn, C. , Flynn, A. , Johnston, K. , Powell, A. , & Scott, A. J. (2020). Image-based sexual abuse: A study on the causes and consequences of non-consensual nude or sexual imagery. London and New York, NY: Routledge.
Hinduja and Patchin, 2010 Hinduja, S. , & Patchin, J. W. (2010). Bullying, cyberbullying, and suicide. Archives of Suicide Research, 14, 206–221.
Lambe et al., 2019 Lambe, L. J. , Della Cioppa, V. , Hong, I. K. , & Craig, W. M. (2019). Standing up to bullying: A social ecological review of peer defending in offline and online contexts. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 45, 51–74.
Latané and Darley, 1968 Latané, B. , & Darley, J. M. (1968). Group inhibition of bystander intervention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 215–221.
Latané and Darley, 1970 Latané, B. , & Darley, J. M. (1970). The unresponsive bystander: Why doesn't he help?. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Latané and Nida, 1981 Latané, B. , & Nida, S. (1981). Ten years of research on group size and helping. Psychological Bulletin, 89(2), 308–324.
Levine and Crowther, 2008 Levine, M. , & Crowther, S. (2008). The responsive bystander: How social group membership and group size can encourage as well as inhibit bystander intervention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(6), 1429–1439.
Levine et al., 2020 Levine, M. , Philpot, R. , & Kovalenko, A. G. (2020). Rethinking the bystander effect in violence reduction training programs. Social Issues and Policy Review, 14(1), 273–296.
Levine et al., 2005 Levine, M. , Prosser, A. , Evans, D. , & Reicher, S. (2005). Identity and emergency intervention: How social group membership and inclusiveness of group boundaries shape helping behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(4), 443–453.
Luxton et al., 2012 Luxton, D. D. , June, J. D. , & Fairall, J. M. (2012). Social media and suicide: A public health perspective. American Journal of Public Health, 102(S2), S195–S200.
Manning et al., 2007 Manning, R. , Levine, M. , & Collins, A. (2007). The Kitty Genovese murder and the social psychology of helping: The parable of the 38 witnesses. American Psychologist, 62(5), 555–562.
Marchant et al., 2017 Marchant, A. , Hawton, K. , Stewart, A. , Montgomery, P. , Singaravelu, V. , Lloyd, K. , … John, A. (2017). A systematic review of the relationship between internet use, self-harm and suicidal behaviour in young people: The good, the bad, and the unknown. PLoS One, 12(8), e0181722. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0181722
McMahon and Banyard, 2012 McMahon, S. , & Banyard, V. L. (2012). When can I help? A conceptual framework for the prevention of sexual violence through bystander intervention. Trauma, Violence, and Abuse, 13(1), 3–14.
McMahon et al., 2017 McMahon, S. , Palmer, J. E. , Banyard, V. , Murphy, M. , & Gidycz, C. A. (2017). Measuring bystander behavior in the context of sexual violence prevention: Lessons learned and new directions. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 32(16), 2396–2418.
Moisuc et al., 2018 Moisuc, A. , Brauer, M. , Fonseca, A. , Chaurand, N. , & Greitemeyer, T. (2018). Individual differences in social control: Who ‘speaks up’ when witnessing uncivil, discriminatory, and immoral behaviours?. British Journal of Social Psychology, 57, 524–546.
Mok et al., 2015 Mok, K. , Jorm, A. F. , & Pirkis, J. (2015). Suicide-related internet use: A review. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 49(8), 697–705.
Moynihan et al., 2015 Moynihan, M. M. , Banyard, V. L. , Cares, A. C. , Williams, L. M. , Potter, S. J. , & Stapleton, J. G. (2015). Encouraging responses in sexual and relationship violence prevention: What program effects remain one year later?. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 30, 110–132.
Nickerson et al., 2014 Nickerson, A. B. , Aloe, A. M. , Livingston, J. A. , & Feeley, T. H. (2014). Measurement of the bystander intervention model for bullying and sexual harassment. Journal of Adolescence, 37, 391–400.
Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2017 Olenik-Shemesh, D. , Heiman, T. , & Eden, S. (2017). Bystanders' behavior in cyberbullying episodes: Active and passive patterns in the context of personal-socio-emotional factors. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 32(1), 23–48.
Palmer et al., 2018 Palmer, J. E. , Nicksa, S. C. , & McMahon, S. (2018). Does who you know affect how you act? The impact of relationships on bystander intervention in interpersonal violence situations. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 33(17), 2623–2642.
Pickering et al., 2018 Pickering, T. A. , Wyman, P. A. , Schmeelk-Cone, K. , Hartley, C. , Valente, T. W. , Pisani, A. R. , … LoMurray, M. (2018). Diffusion of a peer-led suicide preventive intervention through school-based student peer and adult networks. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 598.
Piliavin et al., 1969 Piliavin, I. , Rodin, J. , & Piliavin, J. (1969). Good Samaritanism: An underground phenomenon?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 289–299.
Polanin et al., 2012 Polanin, J. R. , Espelage, D. L. , & Pigott, T. D. (2012). A meta-analysis of school-based bullying prevention programs' effects on bystander intervention behavior. School Psychology Review, 41(1), 47–65.
Powell et al., 2018 Powell, A. , Henry, N. , & Flynn, A. (2018). Image-based sexual abuse. In W. S. DeKeseredy & M. Dragiewicz (Eds.), Routledge handbook of critical criminology. (2nd ed., pp. 305–315). Abingdon and New York, NY: Routledge.
Powell et al., 2020 Powell, A. , Scott, A. J. , Henry, N. , & Flynn, A. (2020). Image-based sexual abuse: An international study of victims and perpetrators. Summary report. Melbourne, VIC: RMIT University.
Pöyhönen et al., 2012 Pöyhönen, V. , Juvonen, J. , & Salmivalli, C. (2012). Standing up for the victim, siding with the bully or standing by? Bystander responses in bullying situations. Social Development, 21, 722–741.
Retter and Shariff, 2012 Retter, C. , & Shariff, S. (2012). A delicate balance: Defining the line between open civil proceedings and the protection of children in the online digital era. Canadian Journal of Law and Technology, 10(2), 232–262.
Rutkowski et al., 1983 Rutkowski, G. K. , Gruder, C. L. , & Romer, D. (1983). Group cohesiveness, social norms, and bystander intervention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(3), 545–552.
Schachter et al., 2016 Schachter, H. L. , Greenberg, S. , & Juvonen, J. (2016). Who's to blame? The effects of victim disclosure on bystander reactions to cyberbullying. Computers in Human Behavior, 57, 115–121.
Shariff and DeMartini, 2016 Shariff, S. , & DeMartini, A. (2016). Cyberbullying and rape culture in universities: Defining the legal lines between fun and intentional harm. In H. Cowie & C.-A. Myers (Eds.), Bullying among university students: Cross-national perspectives (pp. 172–190). New York, NY: Routledge.
Shariff et al., 2012 Shariff, S. , Wiseman, A. , & Crestohl, L. (2012). Defining the lines between children's vulnerability to cyberbullying and the open court principle: Implications of A.B. v. Bragg. Communications Inc. Education and Law Journal, 21(3), 231–262.
Smith et al., 2008 Smith, P. K. , Mahdavi, J. , Carvalho, M. , Fisher, S. , Russell, S. , & Tippett, N. (2008). Cyberbullying: Its nature and impact in secondary school pupils. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(4), 376–385.
Southworth et al., 2007 Southworth, C. , Finn, J. , Dawson, S. , Fraser, C. , & Tucker, S. (2007). Intimate partner violence, technology, and stalking. Violence Against Women, 13, 842–856.
Sticca and Perren, 2013 Sticca, F. , & Perren, S. (2013). Is cyberbullying worse than traditional bullying? Examining the differential roles of medium, publicity, and anonymity for the perceived severity of bullying. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42, 739–750.
Sullivan et al., 2015 Sullivan, E. M. , Annest, J. L. , Simon, T. R. , Luo, F. , & Dahlberg, L. L. (2015). Suicide trends among persons aged 10-24 years United States, 1994-2012. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 64(8), 201–205.
Tennant et al., 2015 Tennant, J. E. , Demaray, M. K. , Coyle, S. , & Malecki, C. K. (2015). The dangers of the web: Cybervictimization, depression, and social support in college students. Computers in Human Behavior, 50, 348–357.
Ting et al., 2011 Ting, L. , Jacobson, J. M. , & Sanders, S. (2011). Current levels of perceived stress among mental health social workers who work with suicidal clients. Social Work, 56(4), 327–336.
Walker and Sleath, 2017 Walker, K. , & Sleath, E. (2017). A systematic review of the current knowledge regarding revenge pornography and non-consensual sharing of sexually explicit media. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 36, 9–24.
Widgerson and Lynch, 2013 Widgerson, S. , & Lynch, M. (2013). Cyber- and traditional peer victimization: Unique relationships with adolescent well-being. Psychology of Violence, 3(4), 297–309.
Woodlock, 2017 Woodlock, D. (2017). The abuse of technology in domestic violence and stalking. Violence Against Women, 23, 584–602.
Wooten, 2017 Wooten, S. C. (2017). Revealing a hidden curriculum of Black women's erasure in sexual violence prevention policy. Gender and Education, 29(3), 405–417.
Wright, 2016 Wright, M. F. (2016). Cyber victimization on college campuses: Longitudinal associations with suicidal ideation, depression, and anxiety. Criminal Justice Review, 41(2), 190–203.
- Prelims
- Technology-Facilitated Violence and Abuse: International Perspectives and Experiences
- Section 1 TFVA Across a Spectrum of Behaviors
- Chapter 1 Introduction
- Chapter 2 Is it Actually Violence? Framing Technology-Facilitated Abuse as Violence
- Chapter 3 “Not the Real World”: Exploring Experiences of Online Abuse, Digital Dualism, and Ontological Labor
- Chapter 4 Polyvictimization in the Lives of North American Female University/College Students: The Contribution of Technology-Facilitated Abuse
- Chapter 5 The Nature of Technology-Facilitated Violence and Abuse among Young Adults in Sub-Saharan Africa
- Chapter 6 The Face of Technology-Facilitated Aggression in New Zealand: Exploring Adult Aggressors' Behaviors
- Chapter 7 The Missing and Murdered Indigenous Women Crisis: Technological Dimensions
- Chapter 8 Attending to Difference in Indigenous People's Experiences of Cyberbullying: Toward a Research Agenda
- Section 2 Text-Based Harms
- Chapter 9 Introduction
- Chapter 10 “Feminism is Eating Itself”: Women's Experiences and Perceptions of Lateral Violence Online
- Chapter 11 Claiming Victimhood: Victims of the “Transgender Agenda”
- Chapter 12 Doxxing: A Scoping Review and Typology
- Chapter 13 Creating the Other in Online Interaction: Othering Online Discourse Theory
- Chapter 14 Text-Based (Sexual) Abuse and Online Violence Against Women: Toward Law Reform?
- Section 3 Image-Based Harms
- Chapter 15 Introduction
- Chapter 16 Violence Trending: How Socially Transmitted Content of Police Misconduct Impacts Reactions toward Police Among American Youth
- Chapter 17 Just Fantasy? Online Pornography's Contribution to Experiences of Harm
- Chapter 18 Intimate Image Dissemination and Consent in a Digital Age: Perspectives from the Front Line
- Section 4 Dating Applications
- Chapter 19 Introduction
- Chapter 20 Understanding Experiences of Sexual Harms Facilitated through Dating and Hook Up Apps among Women and Girls
- Chapter 21 “That's Straight-Up Rape Culture”: Manifestations of Rape Culture on Grindr
- Chapter 22 Navigating Privacy on Gay-Oriented Mobile Dating Applications
- Section 5 Intimate Partner Violence and Digital Coercive Control
- Chapter 23 Introduction
- Chapter 24 Digital Coercive Control and Spatiality: Rural, Regional, and Remote Women's Experience
- Chapter 25 Technology-Facilitated Violence Against Women in Singapore: Key Considerations
- Chapter 26 Technology as Both a Facilitator of and Response to Youth Intimate Partner Violence: Perspectives from Advocates in the Global-South
- Chapter 27 Technology-Facilitated Domestic Abuse and Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Women in Victoria, Australia
- Section 6 Legal Responses
- Chapter 28 Introduction
- Chapter 29 Human Rights, Privacy Rights, and Technology-Facilitated Violence
- Chapter 30 Combating Cyber Violence Against Women and Girls: An Overview of the Legislative and Policy Reforms in the Arab Region
- Chapter 31 Image-Based Sexual Abuse: A Comparative Analysis of Criminal Law Approaches in Scotland and Malawi
- Chapter 32 Revenge Pornography and Rape Culture in Canada's Nonconsensual Distribution Case Law
- Chapter 33 Reasonable Expectations of Privacy in an Era of Drones and Deepfakes: Expanding the Supreme Court of Canada's Decision in R v Jarvis
- Chapter 34 Doxing and the Challenge to Legal Regulation: When Personal Data Become a Weapon
- Chapter 35 The Potential of Centralized and Statutorily Empowered Bodies to Advance a Survivor-Centered Approach to Technology-Facilitated Violence Against Women
- Section 7 Responses Beyond Law
- Chapter 36 Introduction
- Chapter 37 Technology-Facilitated Violence Against Women and Girls in Public and Private Spheres: Moving from Enemy to Ally
- Chapter 38 As Technology Evolves, so Does Domestic Violence: Modern-Day Tech Abuse and Possible Solutions
- Chapter 39 Threat Modeling Intimate Partner Violence: Tech Abuse as a Cybersecurity Challenge in the Internet of Things
- Chapter 40 Justice on the Digitized Field: Analyzing Online Responses to Technology-Facilitated Informal Justice through Social Network Analysis
- Chapter 41 Bystander Apathy and Intervention in the Era of Social Media
- Chapter 42 “I Need You All to Understand How Pervasive This Issue Is”: User Efforts to Regulate Child Sexual Offending on Social Media
- Chapter 43 Governing Image-Based Sexual Abuse: Digital Platform Policies, Tools, and Practices
- Chapter 44 Calling All Stakeholders: An Intersectoral Dialogue about Collaborating to End Tech-Facilitated Violence and Abuse
- Chapter 45 Pandemics and Systemic Discrimination: Technology-Facilitated Violence and Abuse in an Era of COVID-19 and Antiracist Protest